1 / 29

LIN 3098 Corpus Linguistics Lecture 7

LIN 3098 Corpus Linguistics Lecture 7. Albert Gatt. In this lecture. We look at some ways in which corpora can be useful in morphological research. Main focus: morphological productivity. Part 1. Morphology, corpora and productivity. Productivity in linguistics.

ailis
Download Presentation

LIN 3098 Corpus Linguistics Lecture 7

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. LIN 3098 Corpus LinguisticsLecture 7 Albert Gatt

  2. In this lecture • We look at some ways in which corpora can be useful in morphological research. • Main focus: morphological productivity

  3. Part 1 Morphology, corpora and productivity

  4. Productivity in linguistics • The term “productivity” is used in a wide variety of contexts. • Syntactic rules are “productive” in the sense that they can be used to generate new phrases. • The same can be said of some morphological rules.

  5. A definition of productivity • A linguistic process is productive if: • It can be used to produce novel forms. • If a rule is productive, then: • Novel forms (previously unheard) can be understood and produced; • There is no need to store all forms in the mental lexicon.

  6. A couple of examples • Imagine an English adjective garmy. How would you derive a noun out of this adjective? • Many speakers might say garminess • This suggests that –ness suffixation is a productive derivational process. • E.g. Imagine a Maltese verb intoffa. How would you produce a noun from it? • Speakers might say intoffar or inttofamentor intoffazzjoni • This suggests that –ar and –mentsuffixation are productive derivational processes in Maltese.

  7. Productive vs non-productive • Some morphological processes or categories seem to have greater potential to form new words than others • e.g. English -able, -ness • compare to English –th: warmth, strength… (much less productive)

  8. Classical approaches to productivity • Jackendoff (1975): • morphological rules are called redundancy rules: • They capture the relationship between related forms • E.g. Warm  warmth (ADJ  N via addition of –th) • E.g. Desire  desirable (N  ADJ via addition of –able) • If a rule is productive, then it can be used to create novel forms. • e.g. adjectives with –ablecan beproduced “online”

  9. Features of classical approaches • Relies on a binary distinction (un/productive) • Productive rules are typically regular & sub-regularities not considered much (Dressler 2003) • Most of these approaches do not look at corpus data

  10. Productive vs regular • Usually, productive morphological rules are regular. Irregular forms are likely to be stored in the lexicon. • However, we can sometimes detect “sub-regularities”: • sing-sang • ring-rang • bring-brang (?) • Speakers can sometimes generalise these sub-regular processes, perhaps by analogy. • What’s the past tense of tring or spling?

  11. “Possible” vs “attested” • Our tentative definition of productivity focuses on production of novel forms. • By definition, novel forms are: • Possible words of the language; • Previously unattested. • This would suggest that we can’t use corpora to study productivity. • Corpora only contain attested forms.

  12. The problem of frequency • Suppose we find that a corpus contains lots of words ending in some suffix –X. • This doesn’t necessarily imply that the -X suffix is productive. • It could have been productive in the past, but is not anymore. • Therefore, the likelihood of a new word ending in –X is low, despite the high frequency.

  13. Getting around the problem • Frequency can’t give us all the answers. However, one interesting solution is to look at hapaxlegomena. • A corpus will usually contain lots of words occurring only once. • We can think of hapaxes as “one-offs”. • It seems likely that some hapaxes will be “new formations” • NB We can only make this assumption if the corpus is very large.

  14. Corpus-based approaches • View productivity as a gradable phenomenon: • some forms become ingrained through frequent usage • category can still be productive to some extent • productivity estimated in terms of a category’s potential to produce new forms • can account for sub-regularities: productivity of a category is due to a lot of factors, including analogy to existing words

  15. The continuum ADJ+ness Noun ADJ+th Noun lexicalised word Productive morphological process • Productive processes tend to: • be compositional • result in a lot of new words

  16. Why is productivity interesting? • No finite lexicon can contain all words of a language at a certain time • productive processes can be exploited to parse new/unseen lexical items • this is helped by the compositionality of productive processes • can also help to distinguish creative neologism from systematic rule-application. compare: • well-defined, well-intentioned, well-specified • lots of adjectives with a well- prefix • YouTube • a one-off

  17. Theoretical implications • raises interesting questions about the relationship between corpus-based measures and psycholinguistic data • likelihood of a morphological process being applied depends on style, genre, speech community… • can give an indication of language change over time (some processes are fossilised, others become more productive)

  18. Statistical measures of productivity (Baayen 2006)

  19. What we need • A measure of productivity of a process/category C should reflect: • our intuitions about how frequently we encounter C • how easily native speakers can form new words using C • Is it easier to produce a noun with –th (like warmth) or one with –ness (like goodness)?

  20. An analogy • We can compare morphological processes to companies. • All try to dominate a market where the number of clients (words) is limited. • Productivity reflects the extent to which these companies: • have managed to dominate in the past (how many words they’ve formed) • are expanding into new areas of the market (how many new words they’re forming) • may expand in the future (how many as yet unseen words they’re going to form)

  21. Realised productivity (RP) • Given a morphological category C, RP gives a rough indication of the past utility of C in forming new words. • Measured as the number of distinct types in C in a corpus of size N • E.g. regular past tense –ed displays many more types than sub-regular forms such as keep-kept/sleep-slept

  22. Realised productivity cont/d • Why types, not tokens? • Productive processes have lots of types which are hapaxes, or are very infrequent (low token frequency). • Words formed from irregular processes tend to be very frequent (have high token frequency). • Some limitations: • a high RP for a category does not imply that it will keep forming lots of new words • RP is heavily dependent on corpus size

  23. Expanding productivity (P*) • P* gives a rough indication of the rate of expansion of C. • Focuses on the number of hapaxes produced using C in the corpus. • aka hapax-conditioned productivity • NB: P* is still heavily dependent on corpus size!

  24. Potential productivity (P) • Gives an indication of how likely a category C is to form new words in future. • I.e. the potential for C to be already saturated • aka category-conditioned productivity

  25. Some more on P • Unlike RP and P*, P is not very sensitive to corpus size as such • However, very sensitive to frequency of the category. • e.g. if C is realised only once in a corpus of size N, then P = 1! • Recent empirical work has shown that RP and P* may correlate very strongly, but both exhibit a weak correlation with P (Vegnaduzzo 2009) • pattern non-X has high RP and P*, but low P • pattern X-ish has low RP and P*, but high P

  26. P vs. RP and P* • A category C can have low RP and P*, but high P. • In this case, C hasn’t been used much in the past, but is being used quite productively at the moment. • Corresponds to the “ease” with which new words can be formed using the category. • If category has high RP, it may still be saturated, so have low P.

  27. The psycholinguistic connection • Rule vs. direct access: • To produce a word (e.g. illegal), you can either store it directly, or apply the rule on the fly. • Evidence suggests that frequency of baseform vs. derivation is related to which of the two alternatives apply.

  28. The psycholinguistic connection • Complexity-based affix ordering: • Corpus research: more productive affixes follow less productive ones in word formation • It seems that more highly predictable (low productivity) affixes are processed first. • High productivity may also imply less likelihood of entering into further derivational processes.

  29. Works cited • S. Vegnaduzzo (2009). Morphological productivity rankings of complex adjectives. Proc. NAACL-HLT Workshop on Computational Approaches to Linguistic Creativity. • K. Molinen and S. Pulman (2008). The good, the bad and the unknown: Morphosyllabic sentiment tagging of unseen words. Proc. ACL 2008 • Baayen 2006 linked from web page

More Related