1 / 50

Biostatistics in Practice

Biostatistics in Practice. Session 1: Quantitative and Inferential Issues. Peter D. Christenson Biostatistician http://gcrc.LABioMed.org/Biostat. Session 1 Objectives. General quantitative needs in biological research Statistical software Protocol examples, with statistical sections

Download Presentation

Biostatistics in Practice

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Biostatistics in Practice Session 1: Quantitative and Inferential Issues Peter D. Christenson Biostatistician http://gcrc.LABioMed.org/Biostat

  2. Session 1 Objectives General quantitative needs in biological research Statistical software Protocol examples, with statistical sections Overview of statistical issues using a published paper

  3. General Quantitative Needs • Descriptive: Appropriate summarization to meet scientific questions: e.g., • changes, or % changes, or reaching threshold? • mean, or minimum, or range of response? • average time to death, or chances of dying by a fixed time?

  4. General Quantitative Needs, Cont’d • Inferential: Could results be spurious, a fluke, due to “natural” variations or chance? • Sensitivity/Power: How many subjects are needed? • Validity: Issues such as bias and valid inference are general scientific ones, but can be addressed statistically.

  5. Statistical Software

  6. Professional Statistics Software Package Output Stored data; access-ible. Enter code; syntax.

  7. Typical Statistics Software Package Select Methods from Menus www.ncss.com www.minitab.com Output after menu selection Data in spreadsheet

  8. Microsoft Excel for Statistics • Primarily for descriptive statistics. • Limited output. • No analyses for %s.

  9. www.statcrunch.com Almost Free On-Line Statistics Software • Run from browser, not local. • Can store data, results on statcrunch server. • $5/ 6 months usage.

  10. www.stat.uiowa.edu/~rlenth/Power Free Study Size Software

  11. Protocols

  12. Typical Statistics Section of Protocol 11.1 Overview of study design and goals 11.2 Randomization/treatment assignment 11.3 Study size 11.4 Missing data / subject withdrawal or incompletion 11.5 Definitions / outcomes 11.6 Analysis populations 11.7 Data analysis methods 11.8 Interim analyses

  13. Public Protocol Registration www.clincialtrials.gov www.controlled-trials.com Attempt to allow the public to be aware of studies that may be negative. Many journals now require registration in order to consider future publication.

  14. Public Protocol Registration

  15. Example of Protocol--- Displayed in Class ---

  16. Statistical Issues Subject selection Randomization Efficiency from study design Summarizing study results Making comparisons Study size Attributability of results Efficacy vs. effectiveness Exploring vs. proving

  17. Paper with Common Statistical Issues Case Study:

  18. Selecting Study Subjects

  19. Representative or Random Samples How were the children to be studied selected (second column on the first page)? The authors purposely selected "representative" social classes. Is this better than a "randomly" chosen sample that ignores social class? Often hear: Non-random = Non-scientific.

  20. Case Study: Participant Selection No mention of random samples.

  21. Case Study: Participant Selection It may be that only a few schools are needed to get sufficient individuals. If the % among all possible schools that are lower SES is small, none of these schools may be chosen. So, a random sample of schools is chosen from the lower SES schools, and another random sample from the higher SES schools.

  22. Selection by Over-Sampling It is not necessary that the % lower SES in the study is the same as in the population. There may still be too few subjects in a rare subgroup to get reliable data. Can “over-sample” a rare subgroup, and then weight overall results by proportions of subgroups in the population. The CDC NHANES studies do this.

  23. Randomization

  24. Random Samples vs. Randomization We have been discussing the selection of subjects to study, often a random sample. An observational study would, well, just observe them. An interventional study assigns each subject to one or more treatments in order to compare treatments. Randomization refers to making these assignments in a random way.

  25. Basic Study Designs • Observational • Interventional • Cross sectional vs. longitudinal • Cohort vs. Case-Control

  26. Why Randomize? Plant breeding example: Compare yields of varieties A and B, planting each to 18 plots. Which design is better? Systematic Randomized

  27. Why Randomize? So that groups will be similar except for the intervention (treatment or order of treatment). Case-control studies may not provide this similarity. When enrolling, we will not unconsciously choose an “appropriate” treatment. Minimizes the chances of introducing bias when attempting to systematically remove it, as in plant yield example.

  28. Efficiency from Study Design

  29. Case Study: Crossover Design Each child is studied on 3 occasions under different diets. Is this better than three separate groups of children? Why, intuitively? How could you scientifically prove your intuition?

  30. Case Study: Diet Assignments At each school, randomize equal #s of subjects to each order of 3 diets: Pla = Placebo Wash = Washout (placebo here)

  31. Blocked vs. Unblocked Studies AKA matched vs. unmatched. AKA paired vs. unpaired. Block = Pair = Set receiving all treatments. Set could be an individual at multiple times (pre and post), or left and right arms for sunscreen comparison; twins or family; centers in multi-center study, etc. Blocking is efficient because treatment differences are usually more consistent among subjects than each treatment is.

  32. Potential Efficiency Due to Pairing Unpaired Paired A and B Separate Groups A and B in a Paired Set . . . … . . . . . .. . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . … . .. .. .. . . . . . . . . … . .. .. .. Δ 3 3 …….… …. …….… …. 3 A B A B Δ=B-A

  33. Summarizing Study Results and Making Comparisons

  34. Outcome Measures Generally, how were the outcome measures defined (third page)? They are more complicated here than for most studies. What are the units (e.g., kg, mmol, $, years)? Outcome measures are specific and pre-defined. Aims and goals may be more general.

  35. Summarization / Data Reduction How are the outcome measures summarized? e.g., Table 2:

  36. Case Study: Statistical Comparisons How might you intuitively decide from the summarized results whether the additives have an effect? Different Enough? Clinically? Statistically?

  37. Statistical Comparisons: Figure 3

  38. Statistical Comparisons and Tests of Hypotheses Engineering analogy: Signal and Noise Signal = Diet effect Noise = Degree of precision Statistical Tests: Effect is probably real if signal-to-noise ratio Signal/Noiseis large enough. Importance of reducing “noise”, which incorporates subject variability and N.

  39. Back to Efficiency of Design Unpaired Paired A and B Separate Groups A and B in a Paired Set . . . … . . . . . .. . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . … . .. .. .. . . . . . . . . … . .. .. .. Δ Noise 3 3 …….… …. …….… …. Noise 3 A B A B Δ=B-A Signal = 3

  40. Study Size

  41. Number of Subjects The authors say, in the second column on the fourth page: Intuitively, what should go into selecting the study size? We will make this intuition rigorous in Session 4.

  42. Attributability of Study Results

  43. Other Effects, Potential Biases The top of the second column on the fourth page mentions other effects on diet: The issue here is: Could apparent diet differences (e.g., -0.26 B vs. -0.44 Placebo) be attributable to something else?

  44. Efficacy vs. Effectiveness

  45. Non-Completing or Complying Subjects What is the most relevant group of studied subjects: all randomized, mostly compliant, fully compliant? Study Goal: Scientific effect? Societal impact?

  46. Exploring vs.Proving

  47. Multiple and Mid-Study Analyses Many more analyses could have been performed on each of the individual behavior ratings that are described in the first column of the 3rd page. Wouldn’t it be negligent not to do them, and miss something? Is there a downside to doing them? Should effects be monitored as more and more subjects complete?

  48. Multiple Analyses Parent ADHD … GHA: Global Hyperactivity Aggregate Many Separate Measures Teacher ADHD … Class ADHD … Conner … Torture data long enough and it will confess to something

  49. Mid-Study Analyses Effect Too many analyses, as on previous slide 0 Wrong early conclusion Time→ Number of Subjects Enrolled Need to monitor, but also account for many analyses

  50. Bad Science That Seems So Good • Re-examining data, or using many outcomes, seeming to be due diligence. • Adding subjects to a study that is showing marginal effects; stopping early due to strong results. • Emphasizing effects in subgroups. See NEJM 2006 354(16):1667-1669. • Actually bad? Could be negligent NOT to do these, but need to account for doing them.

More Related