1 / 23

Cross Sectional Designs

Cross Sectional Designs. Research Objective. Study relationships among variables for existing groups. Show direct cause & effect. Explain outcomes after the fact. Type of Design. Cross-Sectional Longitudinal. True Experiment Quasi-Experiment. Explanatory Case Study

kane
Download Presentation

Cross Sectional Designs

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Cross Sectional Designs

  2. Research Objective Study relationships among variables for existing groups Show direct cause & effect Explain outcomes after the fact

  3. Type of Design Cross-Sectional Longitudinal True Experiment Quasi-Experiment Explanatory Case Study Exploratory Case Study

  4. Descriptive Cross-Sectionals • Many cross-sectionals are purely descriptive of the characteristics of a population, often of a single population with no comparison group • Don’t use one of these in this class; it is not acceptable • I will not address these designs further in this class; deVaus’s comments are sufficient

  5. Explanatory Cross-Sectionals • Explanatory cross-sectionals identify the relationships among several (sometimes many) variables within one or more populations • Most use two or more comparison groups, although some use test how well two or more theories explain the relationships within a single group • There are generally numerous predictor variables based on the constructs in one or more theories • There may also be more than one outcome variable

  6. Distinctive Features • Select a representative sample from a population or populations • Groups or populations are defined based on the independent or predictor variables, not groups we create through setting up an intervention or treatment and comparison or control group • We then measure the independent or predictor and outcome or dependent variables • Assess the strength of the relationships among variables through qualitative or quantitative analysis

  7. Did you select one or more characteristics to define the population(s) of interest? Yes No Not an explanatory cross-sectional Did you select a (preferably statistically) representative sample of each population of interest based on the characteristics used to define the population(s)? Yes No Not an explanatory cross-sectional Did you collect information each sample based on the predictor and outcome variables of interest? Yes No Not an explanatory cross-sectional Explanatory Cross-sectional Design

  8. Simple Cross-Sectional with Two or More Comparison Populations • 2 or more existing comparison populations • A single measurement at one point in time • In the diagram P=population, RS=representative sample, M=measurement of variables Define P1 RS from P1 M1 Define P2 RS from P2 M1 Define P3 RS from P3 M1

  9. Example of Simple Design • Research question: How does time between marriage and birth of first child affect marital satisfaction of women?

  10. Population & Comparison Groups • Required Characteristics: Married women (not divorced or separated; women who have been married only once; and women who have been married less than 10 years • These are screening criteria and you use them to try to control other factors that could affect marital satisfaction that are not of interest (previous marriages) • Comparison populations/groups: P1 consists of women who were married less than 2 years when the first child was born; P2 is women who were married 2 or more years when the first child was born

  11. Hypotheses • Primary hypothesis: Marital satisfaction will be lower for women married less than two years when the first child was born (one-tailed). • Other theory-based hypotheses: There would probably be several of these, all based on constructs in the theory of this form • There will be a positive correlation between ……. • Non-theory-based hypotheses: These deal with various characteristics that could affect the relationship between time to birth of first child and marital satisfaction, such as size of the woman’s family, educational level, profession, financial resources

  12. Sample & Measure • Select a statistically representative sample of each population (or, given all the caveats about that, as close as you can get) • Measure for all independent or predictor and dependent or outcome variables (theory-based and other)

  13. Analysis • All analyses can be either statistical or qualitative • Test the primary hypothesis first. If there is NO difference between groups for the outcome, you can treat the population as a single population and single sample. However, I recommend continuing testing as two or more populations. • Then test the other hypotheses. • Most researchers want to create some sort of statistical or qualitative model of the relationships for the comparison groups.

  14. Cross-Sectional withRepeated Measures in Time • Most cross-sectional designs have no time component, but a time component can be added with repeated measures • In cross-sectionals, sampling occurs before each measurement – unlike longitudinal designs the participants at each measurement are different people, a different sample, drawn from the population(s) Define P1 RS1 from P1 M1 RS2 from P1 M2 Define P2 RS1 from P2 M1 RS2 from P2 M2 Define P3 RS1 from P3 M1 RS2 from P3 M2

  15. Multiple Cohort Designs • Multiple cohorts sampled over time allow you to determine how the relationships between variables changes over time, the assumption being that such changes are due to broad societal processes • The cohorts are simply different age groups in many cases • E.g., Financial security over time for people born in 1960-69, 70-79, etc. In these cases, the cohorts are essentially the theoretical populations. In this example, we might assume that the financial turbulence since 2000 has affected these cohorts differently. P1 (60-69)RS from P1 M1 P2 (70-79) RS from P2 M1 P3 (80-89) RS from P3 M1 P4 (90-99) RS from P4 M1

  16. Multiple Measures with Cohorts • In other cases, researchers combine multiple cohorts with multiple measures over time. In our example this would reveal how financial security changes over time for people born in the different decades. We might hypothesize that financial security will decline for all populations over time and that the decline will be greater for P1 than P2, P2 than P3, etc. • Again, remember that we draw a new sample for each measurement. P1 RS1 from P1 M1RS2 from P1 M2 P2 RS1 from P2 M1 RS2 from P2 M2 P3 RS1 from P3 M1 RS2 from P3 M2 P4 RS1 from P4 M1 RS2 from P4 M2

  17. Cross-SectionalBefore & After Some Event • These are almost always retrospective designs and are subject to all the limitations of retrospective designs, which include things like people’s ability to remember past feelings and actions, events coloring or changing how we perceived things prior to the event. • However, sometimes we get an opportunity that we just cannot ignore and we use these. • More commonly, people try to treat these as some form of “quasi-experiment.” They are NOT a quasi-experiment. The “event” was not a planned or intentional intervention. NO intentional poke = NOT an experiment.

  18. Example • An example might be why people did (or did not) vote for a woman for president (Hilary Clinton). The two populations would be those that voted for her (in the primary) and those who did not. • We could use several of the gender theories for this study and I won’t lay all that out here. • Assume we did measurement 1 right after the election. We could add a second measurement now, which would improve our study because we could see if the reasons given for voting for her or not remain stable (remember Gersten from last module; his ideas apply here, too). P1 (yes) RS from P1M1 RS2 from P1 M2 P2 (no) RS from P2 M1 RS2 from P2 M2

  19. Sampling • Both statistical and theoretical generalization depends almost entirely on adequacy of the sample • Usually (not always) need a statistically representative sample with regard to the characteristics of the population that we think may affect the variables (independent and dependent) under study or – given the difficulty in getting a true statistically valid sample, something close to it (review Bernard and Wretman readings from module 6)

  20. Describing the Sample • Describe the sample fully in the results section of your study – the reader must have good information to decide if your sample is adequate or not. • DeVaus calls this “descriptive analysis,” but it’s not really analysis at all, just description. I have no idea why he uses this term. I have never heard it before.

  21. What to Include • The norm is to include demographic data about the sample to show whether it is or is not broadly representative of the population(s) of interest with regard to these characteristics. • But focus on the characteristics of the sample that may affect the outcomes of the study – that’s what really matters in terms of the adequacy of the sample. • Review Gersten in particular (module 11). All of his comments about procedures you can use to establish whether two comparison groups are equivalent in important ways can be used in cross-sectionals. For example, you could use multiple forms of data collection similar to multiple pre-tests.

  22. Take Home Points • Cross-sectionals are not a substitute for experiments or any other design. • They fail to meet all the requirements required to demonstrate direct cause and effect (see Shadish). • If well designed, they may provide tentative indications of causality. • They allow the researcher to explore relationships among several theoretically related variables, which can add significantly to explanatory power.

  23. Cross-sectionals depend on the use of comparison groups to indicate causality. All one group designs are basically descriptive in nature because there is no way to know whether absence of the condition of interest produces the same outcome as presence of the condition without a comparison group. • They depend heavily on a representative, independent sample of each comparison population, preferably a statistically representative sample in many cases. • However, they are over-utilized in much social research, probably because they are relatively quick and inexpensive – especially the single population descriptive study.

More Related