280 likes | 301 Views
Chapter 12 Judgment and Choice. This chapter covers the mathematical models behind the way that consumer decide and choose. We will discuss The detection of sensory information The detection of differences between two things Judgments where consumers compare two things
E N D
Chapter 12 Judgment and Choice • This chapter covers the mathematical models behind the way that consumer decide and choose. We will discuss • The detection of sensory information • The detection of differences between two things • Judgments where consumers compare two things • A model for the recognition of advertisements • How multiple judgments are combined to make a single decision As usual, estimation of the parameters in these models will serve as an important theme for this chapter
There Are Two Different Types of Judgments • Absolute Judgment • Do I see anything? • How much do I like that? • Comparative Judgment • Does this bagel taste better than that one? • Do I like Country Time Lemonade better than Minute Maid? Psychologists began investigating how people answer these sorts of questions in the 19th Century
The Early Concept of a “Threshold” Absolute Detection 1.0 Pr(Detect) .5 0 n Physical measurement Difference Detection 1.0 Pr(n Perceived > n2) .5 0 n1 n2 n3
But the Data Never Looked Like That 1.0 .5 Pr(Detect) 0 n
A Simple Model for Detection si is the psychological impact of stimulus i If si exceeds the threshold, you see/hear/feel it Pr[Detect stimulus i] = Pr[si s0] . We make this assumption ei ~ N(0, 2) so that which then implies We also assume
Our Assumptions Imply That the Probability of Detection Is… (Note missing left bracket in Equation 12.6 in book.) Converting to a z-score we get (Note missing subscript i on the z in book)
Making the Equation Simpler But since the normal distribution is symmetric about 0 we can say:
Graphical Picture of What We Just Did 0 Pr(Detection) 0
A General Rule for Pr(a > 0)Where a Is Normally Distributed For a ~ N[E(a), V(a)] we have Pr [a 0] = [E(a) / V(a)]
So Why Do Detection Probabilities Not Look Like a Step Function?
Draw sj Draw si Is si > sj? Assumptions of the Thurstone Model ei ~ N(0, ) Cov(ei, ej) = ij = rij
Deriving the E(si - sj) and V (si - sj) pij = Pr(si > sj ) = Pr(si -sj > 0)
Predicting Choice Probabilities For a ~ N[E(a), V(a)] we have Pr [a 0] = [E(a) / V(a)] Below si - sj plays the role of "a"
Thurstone Case III = 0 = 1 How many unknowns are there? How many data points are there?
Minimum Pearson 2 Same model: Different objective function
Matrix Setup for Minimum Pearson 2 V(p) = V
Modified Minimum Pearson 2 Minimum Pearson 2 Simplifies the derivatives, and reduces the computational time required
Definitions and Background for ML Estimation Assume that we have two possible events A and B. The probability of A is Pr(A), and the probability of B is Pr(B). What are the odds of two A's on two independent trials? Pr(A) • Pr(A) = Pr(A)2 In general the Probability of p A's and q B's would be Note these definitions and identities: fij = npij
ML Estimation of the Thurstone Model According to the Model According to the general alternative
Categorical or Absolute Judgment Love Like Dislike Hate [ ] [ ] [ ] [ ] s1 s2 s3 1 2 3 4 Love Like Dislike Hate Brand 1 Brand 2 Brand 3 .20 .30 .20 .30 .10 .10 .60 .20 .05 .10 .15 .70
Cumulated Category Probabilities Love Like Dislike Hate Brand 1 Brand 2 Brand 3 .20 .30 .20 .30 .10 .10 .60 .20 .05 .10 .15 .70 Raw Probabilities Brand 1 .20 .50 .70 1.00 Cumulated Probabilities Brand 2 .10 .20 .80 1.00 Brand 3 .05 .15 .30 1.00
The Thresholds or Cutoffs c0 = - c4 = + c1 c2 c3 (cJ-1)
A Model for Categorical Data ei ~ N(0, 2) Probability that the discriminal response to item i is less than the upper boundary for category j Probability that item i is placed in category j or less
The Probability of Using a Specific Category (or Less) Pr [a 0] = [E(a) / V(a)] Below ci - sj is plays the role of "a"