1 / 45

Corpora and Statistical Methods – Part 2

Corpora and Statistical Methods – Part 2. Albert Gatt. Preliminaries: Hypothesis testing and the binomial distribution. Permutations. Suppose we have the 5 words {the, dog, ate, a, bone} How many permutations (possible orderings) are there of these words? the dog ate a bone

brent
Download Presentation

Corpora and Statistical Methods – Part 2

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Corpora and Statistical Methods – Part 2 Albert Gatt

  2. Preliminaries: Hypothesis testing and the binomial distribution

  3. Permutations • Suppose we have the 5 words {the, dog, ate, a, bone} • How many permutations (possible orderings) are there of these words? • the dog ate a bone • dog the ate a bone • … • E.g. there are 5! = 120 ways of permuting 5 words.

  4. Binomial coefficient • Slight variation: • How many different choices of three words are there out of these 5? • This is known as an “n choose k” problem, in our case: “5 choose 3” • For our problem, this gives us 10 ways of choosing three items out of 5

  5. Bernoulli trials • A Bernoulli (or binomial) trial is like a coin flip. Features: • There are two possible outcomes (not necessarily with the same likelihood), e.g. success/failure or 1/0. • If the situation is repeated, then the likelihoods of the two outcomes are stable.

  6. Sampling with/out replacement • Suppose we’re interested in the probability of pulling out a function word from a corpus of 100 words. • we pull out words one by one without putting them back • Is this a Bernoulli trial? • we have a notion of success/failure: w is either a function word (“success”) or not (“failure”) • but our chances aren’t the same across trials: they diminish since we sample without replacement

  7. Cutting corners • If the sample (e.g. the corpus) is large enough, then we can assume a Bernoulli situation even if we sample without replacement. • Suppose our corpus has 52 million words • Success = pulling out a function word • Suppose there are 13 million function words • First trial: p(success) = .25 • Second trial: p(success) = 12,999,999/51,999,999 = .249 • On very large samples, the chances remain relatively stable even without replacement.

  8. Binomial probabilities - I • Let π represent the probability of success on a Bernoulli trial (e.g. our simple word game on a large corpus). • Then, p(failure) =1 - π • Problem: What are the chances of achieving success 3 times out of 5 trials? • Assumption: each trial is independent of every other. • (Is this assumption reasonable?)

  9. Binomial probabilities - II • How many ways are there of getting success three times out of 5? • Several: SSSFF, SFSFS, SFSSF, … • To estimate the number of possible ways of getting k outcomes from n possibilities, we use the binomial coefficient:

  10. Binomial probabilities - III • “5 choose 3” gives 10. • Given independence, each of these sequences is equally likely. • What’s the probability of a sequence? • it’s an AND problem (multiplication rule) • P(SSSFF) = πππ(1- π)(1 – π) = π3(1- π)2 • P(SFSFS) = π(1- π) π(1- π) π = π3(1- π)2 • (they all come out the same)

  11. Binomial probabilities - IV • The binomial distribution states that: • given n Bernoulli trials, with probability π of success on each trial, the probability of getting exactly k successes is: probability of each success Number of different ways of getting k successes probability of k successes out of n

  12. Expected value and variance • Expected value: • where π is our probability of success Expected value of X over n trials Variance of X over n trials

  13. Using the t-test for collocation discovery

  14. The logic of hypothesis testing • The typical scenario in hypothesis testing compares two hypotheses: • The research hypothesis • A null hypothesis • The idea is to set up our experiment (study, etc) in such a way that: • If we show the null hypothesis to be false then • we can affirm our research hypothesis with a certain degree of confidence

  15. H0 for collocation studies • There is no real association between w1 and w2, i.e. occurrence of <w1,w2> is no more likely than chance. • More formally: • H0: P(w1 & w2) = P(w1)P(w2) • i.e. P(w1) and P(w2) are independent

  16. Some more on hypothesis testing • Ourresearch hypothesis (H1): • <w1,w2> are strong collocates • P(w1 & w2) > P(w1)P(w2) • Anull hypothesis H0 • P(w1 & w2) = P(w1)P(w2) • How do we know whether our results are sufficient to affirm H1? • I.e. how big is our risk of wrongly falsifying H0?

  17. The notion of significance • We generally fix a “level of confidence” in advance. • In many disciplines, we’re happy with being 95% confident that the result we obtain is correct. • So we have a 5% chance of error. • Therefore, we state our results at p = 0.05 • “The probability of wrongly rejecting H0 is 5% (0.05)”

  18. Tests for significance • Many of the tests we use involve: • having a prior notion of what the mean/variance of a population is, according to H0 • computing the mean/variance on our sample of the population • checking whether the sample mean/variance is different from the sample predicted by H0, at 95% confidence.

  19. The t-test: strategy • obtain mean (x) and variance (s2) for a sample • H0: sample is drawn from a population with mean μ and variance σ2 • estimate the t value: this compares the sample mean/variance to the expected (population) mean/variance under H0 • check if any difference found is significant enough to reject H0

  20. Computing t • calculate difference between sample mean and expected population mean • scale the difference by the variance • Assumption: population is normally distributed. • If t is big enough, we reject H0. The magnitude of t given our sample size N is simply looked up in a table. • Tables tell us what the level of significance is (p-value, or likelihood of making a Type 1 error, wrongly rejecting H0).

  21. Example: new companies • We think of our corpus as a series of bigrams, and each sample we take is an indicator variable (Bernoulli trial): • value = 1 if a bigram is new companies • value = 0 otherwise • Compute P(new) and P(companies) using standard MLE. • H0: P(new companies) = P(new)P(companies)

  22. Example continued • We have computed the likelihood of our bigram of interest under H0. • Since this is a Bernoulli Trial, this is also our expected mean. • We then compute the actual sample probability of <w1,w2> (new companies). • Computet and check significance

  23. Uses of the t-test • Often used to rank candidate collocations, rather than compute significance. • Stop word lists must be used, else all bigrams will be significant. • e.g. M&S report 824 out of 831 bigrams that pass the significance test. • Reason: • language is just not random • regularities mean that if the corpus is large enough, all bigrams will occur together regularly and often enough to be significant. • Kilgarriff (2005): Any null hypothesis will be rejected on a large enough corpus.

  24. Extending the t-test to compare samples • Variation on the original problem: • what co-occurrence relations are best to distinguish between two words, w1 and w1’ that are near-synonyms? • e.g. strong vs. powerful • Strategy: • find all bigrams <w1,w2> and <w1, w2’> • e.g. strong tea, strongsupport • check, for each w1, if it occurs significantly more often with w2, versus w2’. • NB. This is a two-sample t-test

  25. Two-samplet-test: details • H0: For any w1, the probabilities of <w1,w2> and <w1,w2’> is the same. • i.e. μ (expected difference) = 0 • Strategy: • extract sample of <w1,w2> and <w1,w2’> • assume they are independent • compute mean and SD for each sample • compute t • check for significance: is the magnitude of the difference large enough? • Formula:

  26. Simplifying under binomial assumptions • On large samples, variance in the binomial distribution approaches the mean. I.e.: • (similarly for the other sample mean) • Therefore:

  27. Concrete example: strong vs. powerful (M&S, p. 167); NY Times Words occurring significantly more often with powerful than strong Words occurring significantly more often with strong than powerful

  28. Criticisms of the t-test • Assumes that the probabilities are normally distributed. This is probably not the case in linguistic data, where probabilities tend to be very large or very small. • Alternative: chi-squared test (Χ2) • compare differences between expected and observed frequencies (e.g. of bigrams)

  29. The chi-square test

  30. Example • Imagine we’re interested in whether poor performance is a good collocation. • H0: frequency of poor performance is no different from the expected frequency if each word occurs independently. • Find frequencies of bigrams containing poor,performance and poor performance. • compare actual to expected frequencies • check if the value is high enough to reject H0

  31. Example continued OBSERVED FREQUENCIES Expected frequencies need to be computed for each cell: E.g. expected value for cell (1,1) poor performance:

  32. Computing the value • The chi-squared value is the sum of differences of observed and expected frequencies, scaled by expected frequencies. • Value is once again looked up in a table to check if degree of confidence (p-value) is acceptable. • If so, we conclude that the dependency between w1 and w2 is significant.

  33. More applications of this statistic • Kilgarriff and Rose 1998 use chi-square as a measure of corpus similarity • draw up an n (row)*2 (column) table • columns correspond to corpora • rows correspond to individual types • compare the difference in counts between corpora • H0: corpora are drawn from the same underlying linguistic population (e.g. register or variety) • corpora will be highly similar if the ratio of counts for each word is roughly constant. • This uses lexical variation to compute corpus-similarity.

  34. Limitations of t-test and chi-square • Not easily interpretable • a large chi-square or t value suggests a large difference • but makes more sense as a comparative measure, rather than in absolute terms • t-test is problematic because of the normality assumption • chi-square doesn’t work very well for small frequencies (by convention, we don’t calculate it if the expected value for any of the cells is less than 5) • but n-grams will often be infrequent!

  35. Likelihood ratios for collocation discovery

  36. Rationale • A likelihood ratio is the ratio of two probabilities • indicates how much more likely one hypothesis is compared to another • Notation: • c1= C(w1) • c2= C(w2) • c12= C(<w1,w2>) • Hypotheses: • H0: P(w2|w1) = p = P(w2|¬w1) • H1: • P(w2|w1) = p1 • P(w2|¬w1) = p2 • p1 =/= p2

  37. Computing the likelihood ratio

  38. Computing the likelihood ratio • The likelihood (odds) that a hypothesis H is correct is L(H).

  39. Computing the Likelihood ratio • We usually compute the log of the ratio: • Usually expressed as: because, for v. large samples, this is roughly equivalent to a Χ2 value

  40. Interpreting the ratio • Suppose that the likelihood ratio for some bigram <w1,w2> is x. This says: • If we make the hypothesis that w2 is somehow dependent on w1, then we expect it to occur x times more than its actual base rate of occurrence would predict. • This ratio is also better for sparse data. • we can use the estimate as an approximate chi-square value even when expected frequencies are small.

  41. Concrete example: bigrams involving powerful (M&S, p. 174) Source: NY Times corpus (N=14.3m) Note: sparse data can still have a high log likelihood value! Interpreting -2 log l as chi-squared allows us to reject H0, even for small samples (e.g. powerful cudgels)

  42. Relative frequency ratios • An extension of the same logic of a likelihood ratio • used to compare collocations across corpora • Let <w1,w2> be our bigram of interest. • Let C1 and C2 be two corpora: • p1 = P(<w1,w2>) in C1 • p2 = P(<w1,w2>) in C2. • r= p1/p2 gives an indication of the relative likelihood of <w1,w2> in C1 and C2.

  43. Example application • Manning and Schutze (p.176) compare: • C1: NY Times texts from 1990 • C2: NY Times texts from 1989 • Bigram <East,Berliners> occurs 44 times in C2, but only 2 times in C1, so r = 0.03 • The big difference is due to 1989 papers dealing more with the fall of the Berlin Wall.

  44. Summary • We’ve now considered two forms of hypothesis testing: • t-test • chi-square • Also, log-likelihood ratios as measures of relative probability under different hypotheses. • Next, we begin to look at the problem of lexical acquisition.

  45. References • M. Lapata, S. McDonald & F. Keller (1999). Determinants of Adjective-Noun plausibility. Proceedings of the 9th Conference of the European Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics, EACL-99 • A. Kilgarriff (2005). Language is never, ever, ever random. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory 1(2): 263 • Church, K. and Hanks, P. (1990). Word association norms, mutual information and lexicography. Computational Linguistics 16(1).

More Related