1 / 20

Concurrent mobile (cellular) phone use and driving performance:

D.E. Haigney a,b,* , R.G. Taylor c , S.J. Westerman c Transportation Research Part F 3 (2000) 113±121. Concurrent mobile (cellular) phone use and driving performance: task demand characteristics and compensatory processes. 學生 . 莊靖玟. Purpose.

bryar-orr
Download Presentation

Concurrent mobile (cellular) phone use and driving performance:

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. D.E. Haigney a,b,*, R.G. Taylor c, S.J. Westerman cTransportation Research Part F 3 (2000) 113±121 Concurrent mobile (cellular) phone use and driving performance: task demand characteristics and compensatory processes 學生.莊靖玟

  2. Purpose • This paper reports one such simulator study in which driving performance with and without concurrent mobile phone use was compared. Company Logo

  3. Reference • This is consistent with the finding that, in this circumstance, both heart rate and self-report workload increase. (Fairclough, Ashby, Ross, & Parkes, 1991) Company Logo

  4. Reference • The combined level of demand associated with the mobile phone task and the driving task was not sufficient to overload the system. (Briem,1995 & Hedman,1995) Company Logo

  5. Reference • Several studies have found that drivers reduce speed when using a mobile phone. (Alm & Nilsson, 1990; Brookhuis, De Vries, & De Waard, 1991; Fairclough et al.,1991). Company Logo

  6. Reference • A strong version of this hypothesis might refer to a process of risk homeostasis. (see Wilde, 1982, 1988; although also see Adams, 1988, for counter-arguments) • Differences in driver workload have been noted between transmission types. (Zeier, 1979) Company Logo

  7. Reference • Very often studies of mobile phone use and driving performance have failed to consider task demand characteristics. (Royal Society for the Prevention of Accidents, 1997a) Company Logo

  8. Method • Thirty participants (13 male, 17 female) • Mean age : 26.93 years. • Each had held a UK manual transmission driving license for private and light goods vehicles (PLG) for at least one year. Company Logo

  9. Method • 63% of the sample had previous experience of using a mobile phone. • 13% of the sample had experience of using a hands-free phone. • 20% of the sample used a mobile phone while driving with a frequency of once per week or greater. Company Logo

  10. Method • Aston Driving Simulator (ADS). • A 21in. Monitor. • A Nokia 1611 GSM. • A Pulse Coach-3. Company Logo

  11. Method • Dependent measures: * mean speed, *standard deviation of accelerator pedal travel, *brake pedal travel, *number of gear changes were logged every 0.5 s. *number of overtakes, *number of off-road excursions (OFFS), *number of collisions Company Logo

  12. Method • The task used to simulate a mobile phone conversation was developed from the `grammatical reasoning test' detailed by Baddeley (1968) . • The task used previously to replicate the demands of a conversation held over a mobile phone whilst driving (Brown et al., 1969). Company Logo

  13. Method • Participants were presented with five stimulus letters, followed by a statement regarding the relative ordering of two pseudo-randomly selected letters. • Participants were required to indicate whether this statement was true or false. Company Logo

  14. Method • Participants initially completed a short questionnaire. • Participants' resting heart rate was assessed. This was used as a baseline. • A 150 s practice period then followed in which participants were allowed to familiarise themselves with the simulator. Company Logo

  15. Method • Participants then completed four simulated drives: *manual transmission setup × 2 *automatic transmission setup × 2 • Each simulated drive comprised three 150 s periods: *pre-call *during call *post-call • Phone type: *hand-held mobile phone *hands-free mobile phone Company Logo

  16. Results Company Logo

  17. Results Company Logo

  18. Results • 2 (manual vs automatic transmission) ×2 (PHONE). • This only applied to the `during‘ phone call time period. • There were no significant main or interactive effects. Company Logo

  19. Discussion • The results of this study also supported the hypothesis that drivers engage in compensatory behaviour and attempt to reduce workload when using a mobile phone to enable perceived required safety margins to be achieved. • Consistent with the findings of previous studies (Alm & Nilsson, 1990; Brookhuis et al., 1991; Fairclough et al., 1991). Company Logo

  20. Conclusions • The results of the reported study suggest that using a mobile phone while driving may have implications for safety margins that will not be immediately apparent. • It can be predicted that this will render drivers less able to cope with emergency situations or other abrupt increases in driving task demands. • Negative effects were particularly apparent when participants were using a hand-held phone unit. • It is recommended that drivers do not engage in concurrent phone use. Company Logo

More Related