1 / 13

Fundamental Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences, 5th edition David C. Howell

Chapter 21 Choosing a Test. Fundamental Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences, 5th edition David C. Howell. © 2003 Brooks/Cole Publishing Company/ITP   . Description. The following are 11 examples taken from Chapter 21.

cinderella
Download Presentation

Fundamental Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences, 5th edition David C. Howell

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Chapter 21 Choosing a Test Fundamental Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences, 5th edition David C. Howell ©2003 Brooks/Cole Publishing Company/ITP   

  2. Chapter 21 Choosing a Test Description • The following are 11 examples taken from Chapter 21. • Intended as class exercise on choosing an appropriate statistical procedure. • References and answers are given in the text.

  3. Chapter 21 Choosing a Test • 21.3 Seligman, Nolen-Hoeksema, Thorton, and Thorton (1990) administered a measure of optimism to the members of a university swim team. They then had everyone swim their best event, and falsely reported that each person had done somewhat worse than they actually did. Half an hour later they had each person swim the same event again, and took as their dependent measure the ratio of the first time to the second. They hypothesized that more optimistic swimmers would maintain or better their (true) first time, whereas pessimists would feel there was little they could do and would do more poorly the second time around. (Their hypothesis was confirmed.)

  4. Chapter 21 Choosing a Test 21.5 Franklin, Janoff-Bulman, and Roberts (1990) looked at the long-term impact of divorce on college students' levels of optimism and trust. They compared students from divorced families and students from intact families. (Note: They found no differences on generalized trust, but children of divorced families showed less optimism about the future of their own marriages.)

  5. Chapter 21 Choosing a Test 21.6 Zaragoza and Mitchell (1996) showed subjects a video tape of a burglary, and then asked them many questions about what happened. Some of the questions were deliberately misleading, such as saying “At the beginning of the scene, a young man dressed in jeans, a t-shirt, and gloves entered the house. Did he enter through the door?” In fact, the young man did not wear gloves. Every subject heard some misleading statements 0 times, other misleading statements once, and still other misleading statements 3 times. The experimenters examined how often subjects later reported that the video actually included those incorrect observations. (As you might expect, the more often the statements were repeated, the more often they were reported as actually occurring.)

  6. Chapter 21 Choosing a Test 21.10 Pihl, Lau, and Assaad (1997) classified subjects as being generally aggressive or not-aggressive. These subjects were randomly assigned to a drunk or sober condition, given significant amounts of alcohol or juice, and were then run in a study in which they both administered and received electrical shock from a fictitious opponent. When sober, subjects in the high aggressive group administered higher shock levels than subjects in the low aggressive group. When intoxicated and highly provoked, both high and low aggressive subjects administered equal amounts of shock. It was the low aggressive subjects who increased the aggressiveness of their behavior when drunk.

  7. Chapter 21 Choosing a Test 21.12 Pope and Yurgelun-Todd (1996) gave a battery of standard neuropsychological tests to light and heavy users of marijuana after both had abstained from marijuana and other drugs for at least 19 hours. What analysis could they use to compare the performance of the two groups? (The results showed that “heavy users displayed significantly greater impairment than light users on attentional/executive functions.”)

  8. Chapter 21 Choosing a Test 21.14 Many foods and beverages, such as coffee and scotch are frequently termed “acquired tastes.” Pliner (1982) was interested in investigating whether familiarity with a flavor leads to greater approval—the “acquired-taste” phenomenon. She had 24 undergraduates taste each of four unfamiliar tropical fruit juices 0, 5, 10, or 20 times. (For each subject she randomized which juice would be tasted how many times.) Subjects were then asked to rate the degree to which they liked the taste of the juice. What statistical procedures are suitable for analyzing these data? What test would she use if she wanted a distribution-free test? (Note: She found the effect that she had expected—greater familiarity led to greater approval.)

  9. Chapter 21 Choosing a Test • 21.15 Chaffin and Imreh (2002) examined the way a professional pianist learned a difficult piece of music. Among other things, they were interested in the relationship between the number of times the pianist rehearsed a specific bar of music (as the dependent variable) and a) the number of specific mutual features (across 10 dimensions) that the pianist associated with that bar, b) the number of notes per bar, and c) – f) four measures reflecting location in the formal structure of the music (e.g. whether it was the first bar in a section of the piece).

  10. Chapter 21 Choosing a Test 21.18 Most people are basically happy. Diener and Diener (1996) reported data on subjective well-being from residents of 43 countries around the world. The mean was 6.33 on a scale from 0 (most unhappy) to 10 (most happy). What would you do with data such as these?

  11. Chapter 21 Choosing a Test 21.19 Vasta, Rosenburg, Knott, and Gaze (1997) compared the performance of four groups of male and females subjects to Piaget’s water-level task, in which a subject looks at a partially full glass of water and then draws where the water level would be if the glass were tilted to the side. The four groups were bartenders, servers, clerical workers, and salespersons. (Contrary to previous research, bartenders and servers were best. Females were generally better than males.)

  12. Chapter 21 Choosing a Test 21.24 Lobel, Dunkel-Schetter, and Scrimshaw (1992) examined medical risk factors, gestational age, and the mother's emotional stress as predictors of low birthweight. How could they assess the relationship between these factors and low birthweight? (Note: Gestational age and stress predicted birthweight but not medical risks. Women who experienced daily anxiety were most likely to deliver low-birthweight babies.)

  13. Chapter 21 Choosing a Test 21.27 Hunt, Streissguth, Kerr, and Olson (1995) asked 14-year-olds to perform a spatial-visual reasoning task where it was possible to evaluate the amount of time each person viewed a figure before responding, and the accuracy of that response. They also obtained the amount of alcohol the child’s mother reported consuming during pregancy (14 years earlier). (Both study time and number correct were negatively correlated with alcohol consumption during pregnancy, though, as expected, the correlations were not very high.)

More Related