310 likes | 538 Views
Like Mother, Like Daughter? A Study of Attachment Style and Explanatory Style Across Generations. Jenica Riehle Beloit College Fall 2006. Purpose. To explore the construct of attachment security in relation to explanatory style.
E N D
Like Mother, Like Daughter? A Study of Attachment Style and Explanatory Style Across Generations Jenica Riehle Beloit College Fall 2006
Purpose • To explore the construct of attachment security in relation to explanatory style. • Few research studies connect secure attachment and explanatory style, and few research studies examine these factors together across generations (i.e. mother and daughter).
What is Attachment? • Attachment is a relationship quality between two people, usually studied between mother and child • In adulthood it is also measured as a style, kind of like a personality trait • Developed through the study of primates and humans, attachment theory was initially based on observations of infant-mother relationships as well as relationships between mothers and young children (Marvin & Britner, 1999).
What is Attachment? • “Attachment behaviour is conceived as any form of behaviour that results in a person attaining or retaining proximity to some other differentiated and preferred individual, who is usually conceived as stronger and/or wiser” (Bowlby, 1979, p. 129).
Attachment History • John Bowlby (1979) ~ explained the theory of attachment in development • Initial mother-infant relationship studies in strange situation by Mary Ainsworth et al. (1971) • Mary Main et al. (1985) devised the Adult Attachment Interview to measure attachment in adults • Other adult attachment measurements were devised (e.g. Bartholomew and Horowitz, 1991)
Main’s AAI ratings • Adults are placed into one of three categories based on their interview: • Autonomous/ Secure- these adults value attachment relationships and think them as being integral parts to the formation of their own personality. Their descriptions are coherent no matter if the subject of discussion is positive or negative. • Dismissing - these adults are more likely to deny the importance of attachment relationships or to value their relationship partners more than themselves without giving a solid reason why. They tend to have a poor memory of childhood experiences. • Preoccupied - these adults are still caught up with thinking through past relationships so are not able to describe them in a coherent way • (Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985)
Self-Report Measurement = Attachment Style • Bartholomew’s (1990) model of the self is dichotomized in either a positive or negative: • the self viewed as worthy of love and attention vs. being unworthy of love an attention • the model of the attachment figure is either positive or negative: • the other is viewed as available and caring vs. viewed as rejecting, distant, and uncaring (cited in Feeney & Noller, 1996). table (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991, p. 227)
Measurement- Relationship Questionnaire 1. “It is easy for me to become emotionally close to others. I am comfortable depending on them and having them depend on me. I don’t worry about being alone or having others not accept me.” Secure 2. “I am uncomfortable getting close to others. I want emotionally close relationships, but I find it difficult to trust others completely, or to depend on them. I worry that I will be hurt if I allow myself to become too close to others.” Fearful 3. “I want to be completely emotionally intimate with others, but I often find that others are reluctant to get as close as I would like. I am uncomfortable being without close relationships, but I sometimes worry that others don’t value me as much as I value them.” Preoccupied 4. “I am comfortable without close emotional relationships. It is very important to me to feel independent and self-sufficient, and I prefer not to depend on others or have others depend on me.” Dismissive Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1990
What is explanatory style? • “[Explanatory style is one’s tendency to offer similar sorts of explanations for different events” (Peterson, Buchanan, & Seligman, 1995, p. 1 ) • Positive explanation + negative event = external, unstable, and specific: I didn’t get the job because the interviewer was having a bad day and was not able to pay attention to my answers • Negative explanation + negative event = internal, stable, and global: I didn’t get the job because I never know what to say in an interview and I never try hard enough • Negative explanation + positive event = external, unstable, and specific: I was hired for the job because the boss was having a good day and thought I could do the job • Positive explanation + positive event = internal, stable, and global: I was hired for the job because I had worked hard during the training for it and was the best one qualified to do the job (Peterson, Buchanan, & Seligman, 1995).
Measurement • The Attributional Style Questionnaire (ASQ) is a self-report measurement developed by Seligman et al. in 1979 (see Peterson, Buchanan, & Seligman, 1995). • The questionnaire describes 12 specific events: • 6 positive, e.g. “you become very rich,” • 6 negative e.g. “you cannot get all the work done that others expect you to do” • The explanations are rated on the three different variables: internal/external, stable/unstable, global/specific
Attachment style, Explanatory style, Mothers, and Daughters • In studies involving both measurements: • Secure attachment styles have predicted positive or optimistic outlooks on life while insecure attachment styles have predicted negative or pessimistic outlooks on life (Hjelle et al., 1996; Greenberger & Mclaughlin, 1998; Salzman, 1996; Laible, Carlo, & Roesch, 2004) • Secure males have greater coping strategies while secure females have greater optimism scores (Greenberger & McLaughlin, 1998) • In studies looking at college-age women and their mothers: • Mothers and daughters tend to have similar responses to social situations (Stednitz & Epkins, 2006)
Current Study • The current study measured and compared the attachment styles and explanatory styles of college-aged women and their primary care-giving mothers.
Hypotheses It was hypothesized that: • 1. secure attachment style would predict a positive explanatory style in all participants. Insecure attachment style was predicted to be associated with a negative explanatory style. • 2. the mothers’ attachment style would correspond with the daughters’ attachment style. • 3. the mothers’ explanatory style would correspond with the daughters’ explanatory style. • Furthermore, there was an interest in looking at how a mother’s attachment style and explanatory style interacted to affect a daughter’s explanatory style.
Method • Participants: 44 female college students between the ages of 17 and 22 (mean age = 19.32, SD = 1.36) from a liberal arts college in the Midwest as well as their mothers between the ages of 44 and 61 (mean age = 51.27, SD = 3.89). 77% of the mothers responded which produced data for 44 mother-daughter dyads.
Measurements: • A demographics questionnaire • The Relationship Questionnaire (RQ) • a self-report questionnaire developed by Bartholomew and Horowitz (1991) and was used to measure attachment behaviors using descriptions of personal feelings about personal relationships. • The Attributional Style Questionnaire (ASQ) • a self-report questionnaire developed by Peterson et al. (1982) which describes twelve hypothetical situations (six bad events and six good events) and was used by asking participants to give causes for each event. Participants are also asked to rate these causes in internality vs. externality, stability vs. instability, and globality vs. specificity on a 7-point scale.
Procedure: • A screening question was asked about primary caregiver: When you were younger which of your parents spent the most time caring for you? The participants whose mothers were the primary caregivers were allowed to continue with the survey. • Student participants filled out the surveys on campus. After filling out the surveys each participant gave contact information for her mother. Survey questionnaires were mailed to the mothers.
Results Mean ASQ score: • whole group: 1.69 (SD = .335) • Mothers: 1.71 (SD = .527) • Daughters: 1.67 (SD = .412) Attachment Style: • Mothers: Daughters: 61.4% Secure 25.0% Secure 6.8% Fearful 38.7% Fearful 11.3% Preoccupied 22.7% Preoccupied 20.5% Dismissive 13.6% Dismissive • A Pearson Chi-square test showed that mothers as a group were different from daughters as a group, {χ2(3, N=88 ) = 18.804, p < .001}
Results (continued) • A One-way ANOVA: Explanatory Style by Attachment Style • An association between the two measurements was revealed, F (4, 84) = 6.52, p <.001. • Participants who rated themselves as Secure had the highest Attributional Style score with a mean of 3.26 (SD = 2.42). • Those participants who rated themselves as Preoccupied and Dismissive were not significantly different from each other with a mean of 1.24 and 1.82 (SD = 3.10 and 2.05) respectively. • Those participants who rated themselves as Fearful had the lowest Attributional Style score with a mean of .375 (SD = 2.49).
Results (continued) • A Tukey HSD test: • those participants who rated themselves as Secure are different from those rating themselves as Fearful and Preoccupied, p< .000 and p< .046 respectively. Those participants who rated themselves as Dismissive were not significantly different from the other three.
A Pearson Chi-Square test looking at mother Attachment Style by daughter Attachment Style showed no significant relationship between the two, but inspection of the data reveals that secure mothers were more likely to have fearful daughters than secure daughters. • Contrary to prediction, mothers and daughters were not similar in attachment.
Additional Analysis • A Two-way ANOVA looking at group (mothers and daughters) and attachment to predict Explanatory Style: • revealed a corrected model significance, F (7, 80) = 2.78, p = .012, with attachment style being the only significant predictor of Explanatory Style score, F (3, 84) = 3.65, p = .016. • The group a participant belonged to was not a significant predictor of Explanatory Style score, meaning that mothers and daughters did not differ in level of optimism.
A follow-up T-test revealed that mothers were more optimistic than daughters were {t (86) = -2.038, p = .045} • There was no significant correlation between mothers’ explanatory style and daughters’ explanatory style within dyads. • Daughters were not like mothers in either attachment style or explanatory style.
Discussion • Some Hypotheses were supported, while others were not • Daughters’ insecure attachment style possibly due to divorced parents • Mothers’ more optimistic style based on different generational factors
Conclusion • A crucial first step at attempting to associate attachment style with explanatory style • Further studies • Findings of this study • More support could be given to college students who are going through the transitional college experience
References • Ainsworth, M. D., Bell, S. M., & Stayton, D. J. (1971). Individual differences in strange-situation behaviour of one-year-olds. In Schaffer, H. R. (1971). The origins of human social relations. Oxford, England: Academic Press. • Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44(4), 709-716. • Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1993). A psychometric study of the adult attachment interview: Reliability and discriminate validity. Developmental Psychology, 29(5), 870-879. • Bartholowmew, K. & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test of a four category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(2), 227-244. • Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. London: Tavistock Publications. • Brewin, C. & Andrews, B. (1996). Intergenerational links and positive self-cognitions; Parental correlates of optimism, learned resourcefulness, and self-evaluation. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 20(3), 247-263. • Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.). (1999). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. New York: The Guilford Press. • Crowell, J. A., Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P.R. (1999). Measurement of individual differences in adolescent and adult attachment. In Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 434-465) New York: The Guilford Press. • Charles, D. R., PhD, & Charles, M., PhD. (2006). Sibling loss and attachment style: An exploratory study. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 23(1), 72-90. • Davila, J., Burge, D., & Hammen, C. (1997). Why does attachment style change? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(4), 826-838. • Davis, S. F., Hanson, H., Edson, R., Ziegler, C. (1992). The relationship between optimism-pessimism, loneliness, and level of self-esteem. College Student Journal, 26(2), 244-247. • Drill, R. (1986). Young adult children of divorced parents; Depression and the perception of loss. Journal of Divorce, 10(1), 169-187. • Greenberger, E. & McLaughlin, C. S. (1998). Attachment, coping, and explanatory style in late adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 27(2), 121-139. • Heinonen, K., Räikkönen, K., Keltikangas-Järvinen, L. (2005). Self-esteem in early and late adolescence predicts dispositional optimism-pessimism in adulthood: A 21-year longitudinal study. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(3), 511-521. • Hesse, E. (1999). The adult attachment interview: Historical and current perspectives. . In Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 395-433) New York: The Guilford Press. • Hesse, E. & Main, M. (2000) Disorganized infant, child, and adult attachment: Collapse in behavioral and attentional strategies. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 48(4), 1097-1229. • Higgins, N.C. & Hay, J. L. (2003). Attributional style predicts causes of negative life events on the attributional style questionnaire. The Journal of Social Psychology, 143(2), 253-271. • Hjelle, L. A., Busch, E. A., & Warren, J. E. (1996). Explanatory style, dispositional optimism, and reported parental behavior. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 57(4). 489-600. • Kilmann, P. R., Carranza, L. V., & Vendemia J. M. C. (2006). Recollections of parent characteristics and attachment patterns for college women of intact vs. non-intact families. Journal of Adolescence, 29(1), 89-102 Laible, D. J., Carlo, G, & Roesch, S. C. (2004). Pathways to self esteem in late adolescence: The role of parent and peer attachment, empathy, and social behaviors. Journal of Adolescence, 27(6), 703-716. • Levine, L. V., Tuber, S. B., Slade, A., & Ward, M. J. Mother’s mental representations and their relationship to mother-infant attachment. Bullitin of the Menninger Clinic, 55(4), 454-470. • Lopez, G. F., & Gormley, B. (2002). Stability and change in adult attachment style over the first-year college transition: Relations to self-confidence, coping, and distress patterns. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 49(3), 355-364. • Lopez, G. F., Melendez, M. C., & Rice, K. G. (2000). Parental divorce, parent-child bonds, and adult attachment orientations among college students: a comparison of three racial/ethnic groups. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47(2), 177-186. • Love, K. M. & Murdock, T. B. (2004). Attachment to parents and psychological well-being: An examination of young adult college students in intact families and stepfamilies. Journal of Family Psychology, 18(4), 600-608. • Main, M. (1996). Introduction to the special section on attachment and psychopathology: 2. Overview of the field of attachment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(2), 237-243. • Main, M. & Hesse, E. (1990). Parents' unresolved traumatic experiences are related to infant disorganized attachment status: Is frightened and/or frightening parental behavior the linking mechanism? In Greenberg, M. T., Cicchetti, D., & Cummings, E. M. (Eds). (1990). Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention. The John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation series on mental health and development. (pp. 161-182). Chicago, IL, US: University of Chicago Press.
References Continued • Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1-2), 66-104. • Martinez, R. & Sewell, K. W. (2000). Explanatory style in college students: Gender differences and disability status. College Student Journal, 34(1), 72. • Marvin, R. S. & Britner, P. A. (1999). Normative development: The ontogeny of attachment. . In Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 44-67) New York: The Guilford Press. • Nielsen, L. (1999). College aged students with divorced parents: Facts and Fiction. College Student Journal, 33(4), 543-573. • Peterson, C. & Villanova, P. (1988). An expanded attributional style questionnaire. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97(1), 87-89. • Peterson, C., Buchanan, G. M., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1995). Explanatory Style. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. • Rohner, R. P. (2004). The parental “acceptance-rejection syndrome”: Universal correlates of perceived rejection. American Psychologist, 59(8), 830-840. • Salzman, J. P. (1996). Primary attachment in female adolescents: Association with depression, self-esteem, and maternal identification. Psychiatry, 59, 20-33. • Seligman, M. E. P. & Schulman, P. (1986). Explanatory style as a predictor of productivity and quitting among life insurance sales agents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(4), 832-838. • Snyder, C. R. & Lopez, S. J. (2005). Handbook of Positive Psychology. Oxford: University Press. • Stayton, D. J., Ainsworth, M. D., & Main, M. B. (1973) Development of separation behavior in the first year of life: Protest, following, and greeting. Developmental Psychology, 9(2), 213-225. • Stednitz, J. N. & Epkins, C. C. (2006). Girls’ and mothers’ social anxiety, social skills, and loneliness: Associations after accounting for depressive symptoms. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 35(1), 148-154. • Symister, P. & Friend, R. (2003). The influence of social support and problematic support on optimism and depression in chronic illness: A prospective study evaluating self-esteem as a mediator. Health Psychology, 22(2), 123-130. • Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1995). Of the way we are: On temperament, attachment, and the transmission gap: A rejoinder to Fox (1995). Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 411-415. • Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1995). Adult attachment representations, parental responsiveness and infant attachment: A meta-analysis on the predictive validity of the Adult Attachment Interview. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 387-403. • Ward, M. J., Lee, S. S., & Lipper, E. G. (2000). Failure-to-thrive is associated with disorganized infant-mother attachment and unresolved maternal attachment Infant Mental Health Journal, 21(6), 428-442. • Wallerstein, J. S., Lewis, J. M., & Blakeslee, S. (2000). The Unexpected Legacy of Divorce: A Twenty-Five-Year Landmark Study. New York: Hyperion. • Wilkinson, R. B. (2004). The role of parental and peer attachment in the psychological health and self-esteem of adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 33(6), 479-493.
Acknowledgments • I would like to thank: Suzanne Cox and Whitney Sweeney for being wonderful advisors and reading my thesis and helping with my research, Anthony Domanico, my Fiancé, for sticking by my side through all the long nights, as well as reading multiple drafts The Psychology Department for funding my project, The participants, daughters and mothers, for giving me their time, And my parents and sisters for reading my thesis on their vacation time and giving great criticism.