1 / 51

Disorders of Syntax and Morphology

Ling 411 – 08. Disorders of Syntax and Morphology. Goodglass 1993: Chapter 6. Major Language Areas. Supramarginal gyrus (Goldstein’s area). Angular gyrus (Geschwinds’s area ). Wernicke’s area. Exner’s area Broca’s area. Superior parietal lobule. Agrammatism.

dino
Download Presentation

Disorders of Syntax and Morphology

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Ling 411 – 08 Disorders of Syntax and Morphology Goodglass 1993: Chapter 6

  2. Major Language Areas Supramarginal gyrus (Goldstein’s area) Angular gyrus (Geschwinds’s area) Wernicke’s area Exner’s area Broca’s area Superior parietal lobule

  3. Agrammatism • Usually associated with Broca’s aphasia • Generally present in Broca’s aphasia • But other aphasics also have grammatical dysfunctions • Paragrammatism – common in Wernicke aphasia • A lot of variation among different patients

  4. Agrammatism vs. Paragrammatism • Paragrammatism – too much speech • Normal or excessive fluency • Use of inappropriate words • Neologisms • No lack of function words and inflections • But not always used appropriately • Common in Wernicke’s aphasia • Agrammatism – not enough speech • Lack of fluency • Omission (NOT deletion!) of function words and inflections • Common in Broca’s aphasia

  5. Omission vs. Deletion • Goodglass (106): • Sentences with a deleted main verb (“Joan and I . . . Coffee”) may continue to appear. • . . . misuse or deletions of morphology . . . • Is he talking about deletion or omission? • Deletion implies that it was first there, and then removed • Omission – it wasn’t put in at all • Goodglass is following a practice that was common among linguists at the time he wrote the book

  6. Broca’s Aphasia • Damage to frontal lobe • Mainly, inferior frontal gyrus • Largely intact comprehension • Nonfluent, agrammatic speech • “Telegraphic speech” – • Abundance of content words (e.g., nouns) • Lack of function words (e.g. prepositions) • Impaired verb processing • Bates, Chen, Tzeng, Li & Opie, 1991; Damasio & Tranel, 1993; Daniele, Giustolisi, Silveri, Colosimo & Gainotti, 1994; Lamb & Zhang, 2010; Shapiro & Caramazza, 2003

  7. Verbal short-term memory deficit(in Broca aphasia) • Patients can readily point to individual objects or body parts named by the examiner • But when asked to point to the same items in a specific sequence they often fail at the level of only two or three items Benson & Ardila 124 How to explain?

  8. Subtypes of Broca aphasia • Type I • A.k.a. little Broca aphasia • Milder defects • Less extensive damage • Better prognosis • Type II • Symptoms worse • More extensive damage • These are not distinct, but variations • Two spans along a scale

  9. Agrammatism: an early observation (1819) • Deleuze (1819), referring to a French-speaking patient: The patient “used exclusively the infinitive of verbs and used no pronouns. … She produced absolutely no conjugated verb.” Goodglass 1993: 104

  10. Example of agrammatic speech Examiner: Can you tell me about why you came back to the hospital? Patient: yes … eh … Monday … eh … dad … Peter Hogan and dad … hospital. Er … two … er … doctors … and … er … thirty minutes … and ... er … yes … hospital. And … er … Wednesday … Wednesday. Nine o’clock. And … er … Thursday, ten o’clock … doctors … two … two … doctors… and … er … teeth … fine. E: Not exactly your teeth … your g- P: Gum … gum … E: What did they do to them? P: And er … doctor and girl … and er … and er gum … Goodglass 1993: 107

  11. Some features of agrammatism • Telegraphic speech • Short utterances • Omission of grammatical functors • Relative abundance of substantives • Verbs are uncommon, rare in some patients • When present, uninflected or –ing form • For French aphasics, infinitive form • Use of word order is generally spared • Comprehension is impaired for complex sentences

  12. Problems in the study of agrammatism • Must be distinguished from paragrammatism • Grammatical aberrations – even among Broca aphasics – vary from patient to patient • Linguistics has not (yet) provided clear answers to important basic questions: • What normal grammatical functions are • How they operate

  13. Syntax • First, we need to dispel the notion that syntax is one capacity, that can be lost (or spared) as a unit • Syntax can be understood as a set of constructions • Learned by children (and others) one by one • Like vocabulary • Some can be lost, others spared, in aphasia • It is a label of the grammarian for multiple things • Word order is often spared in Broca’s aphasia while a lot of syntax is lost

  14. Stability of word order in agrammatism • Agrammatic patients can usually handle word order in both production and comprehension • Evidence (comprehension) • passive sentences misconstrued • The horse was kicked by the dog • Broca’s aphasic: horse as kicker • Passive marker not apprehended • Canonical word order guides the interpretation • Possibly aided by conceptual knowledge

  15. Reading and writing in agrammatism • Agrammatic difficulties are also seen in • Oral reading • Writing to dictation • Repetition • But: • Some patients are agrammatic in speech but not in writing (Goodglass 1993: 110) • Somecan repeat correctly • How to explain? • Menn & Obler (1990) describe some patients who are less agrammatic in oral reading than in spontaneous speech (Goodglass 1993:111)

  16. Variation among agrammatics (Goodglass 1993:107) • Syntax and morphology (study of agrammatic French aphasics) • Some patients have fairly good syntax but defective morphology • Some patients have fairly good morphology but defective syntax • Both types of patients fail to use inflected verb forms • Gleason et al. observations (1975) • Some patients use –s plural marker but not articles • Other patients use articles but not –s plural marker

  17. Loss of the use of relational markers in receptive agrammatism (118) • E.g. father’s sister • Ex: Is “my father’s sister” a man or a woman? • Patient answers randomly • Unable to grasp the relational function of –’s • Command given in testing: • Ex: Touch the comb with the pencil • Patient may touch the pencil with the comb • Perhaps picks up comb because the word comb comes first in the instruction • Locative relations somewhat less fragile • in back of/in front of, over/under, before/after

  18. Linguistic structure in the cortex:What we learn from agrammatism • Agrammatism is generally associated with Broca’s aphasia • Therefore, the grammatical skills lost in Broca’s aphasia must be supported at least in part by either • Broca’s area, or • Area(s) adjacent to Broca’s area • In other words: There must be something in or near Broca’s area that is essential for correct grammatical production • And grammatical comprehension – • Receptive agrammatism

  19. Receptive processing in Broca’s aphasia? • Problem: • Broca’s area is in frontal lobe • Frontal lobe is supposed to be for motor production • Motor production is top-down processing • Receptive functions involve bottom-up processing • Comprehension involves sensory processing • in frontal lobe? • Bottom-up (receptive) processing in frontal lobe?

  20. Receptive agrammatism in Broca’s aphasiaTwo avenues to explanation • The role of short-term memory, and Broca’s area in short-term memory • Maybe the frontal lobe can have receptive function • To explore this possibility we must first examine the phenomenon of imagery

  21. For perspective, A related problem: Imagery • Types of sensory imagery • Visual • Auditory • Somatosensory • Cf. Motor imagery

  22. Visual Imagery • Visual images of people, buildings, etc. • What is a visual image? • What does it consist of? • Is it a little picture? • If so, where are the eyes to see it? • What is it drawn on? • Where is the visual perception system to interpret it? • If not, what?

  23. Auditory Imagery • Auditory images of words, music, etc. • We can hear things in our heads • What is an auditory image? • What does it consist of? • Sound? • There is no air inside the head to vibrate • What hears it? • Little ears inside the head?

  24. How Imagery Operates • It’s unlikely that visual imagery uses some mechanism independent of that for vision • Therefore, it must use (some of) the same neural connections used in perception • For visual imagery, pathways in the occipital lobe • For auditory imagery, pathways in the temporal lobe • For tactile imagery, pathways in the parietal lobe • Imagery is activation of some of the same neural pathways that get activated upon receiving input from sense organs

  25. Anatomical consequences • Consequences of imaging explanation • Top-down processing in perceptual areas • Perceptual pathways must have parallel pathways of opposite direction • Why are imagined scenes less vivid than those resulting from input to the eyes?

  26. Bidirectional Processing • Imagery requires top-down processing • Using pathways that typically operate bottom-up • Therefore, perceptual pathways must generally be bidirectional • Anatomical evidence supports the hypothesis • Reciprocal pairs of cortico-cortical axons

  27. Bidirectional Connections • Most corticocortical connections are bidirectional • An established finding from neuroanatomy • It’s not because the connecting nerve fibers (axons) are themselves bidirectional • It’s because we find different but roughly parallel fibers going in opposite directions

  28. Bidirectional Processing in Frontal Lobe? • Frontal lobe processing: typically top-down • But there is a large amount of uniformity in cortical structure • Hypothesis: Bottom-up processing also in frontal lobe • From perceptual (i.e. posterior) areas to locations in frontal lobe • We already have seen evidence: the arcuate fasciculus

  29. Bidirectional connections in frontal lobe • Would explain how Broca’s area is involved in receptive grammatical processing • Would account for the finding that interpretation of prepositions and verbs is a frontal lobe function • Finding from the study of agrammatism

  30. Attempts to explain agrammatism • Many theories have been proposed • Cf. Goodglass 1993:111ff • Some intriguing ideas • Loss of relational use of words (Jakobson, Luria) • Difficulty with markers of such relationships • Impairment of inner speech (Luria) • Hence, impairment of auditory working memory • Difficulty with unstressed words (Goodglass, Kean) • Substantive words are commonly stressed • Functors are generally unstressed

  31. Caution in interpreting • Agrammatism may not be just one phenomenon • Syntax is not one structure but several • All agrammatics and probably all Broca’s aphasics are deficient in use of verbs • Other phenomena of agrammatism show more variability • The problem (or part of the problem) may not be grammar as such: • Short-term memory – the inner speech loop • Phonology: stressed vs. unstressed words

  32. Phonological factors • Function words are (in general) unstressed • Maybe the difficulty is in production of unstressed words • Intriguing finding of Goodglass et al. • Function words • May be produced after a stressed word • But almost never produced initially • Production starts with stressed word • Even with repetition • Open the door > Open the door • The door is open > Door is open

  33. More evidence on relational markers (119f) • Grammatical particles that do not mark relations are exempt from omission • and • Japanese clause-final particles • Emphatic yo • Question marker ka • Confirmation-seeking particle ne • Verbs always have a syntactic implication • I.e. relationship to one or more nouns • Menn & Obler: Impairment affecting grammatical elements that mark relationships within the sentence

  34. Nouns and Verbs:Back Brain & Front Brain (?) • “A Neurolinguistic Universal” –E. Bates • Verb deficit in Broca’s aphasia • Noun deficit in Wernicke’s aphasia • Suggests that • Verbs are represented in frontal lobe • Nouns are represented in or near temporal lobe – angular gyrus and/or supramarginal gyrus) and/or middle temporal gyrus • Supports what we derive from the proximity principle

  35. Proceed with Caution! • We already know that a noun or a verb has a complex cortical representation • Therefore it is not in a single location • Rather, a functional web • So what are we talking about? • The cardinal node of the functional web of a noun • The cardinal node of the functional web of a verb

  36. A patient with non-fluent aphasia • Patient ROX (McCarthy & Warrington 1985) • Impaired production and comprehension of verbs • Excellent production and comprehension of nouns • Had no difficulty imitating common actions • But had difficulty in performing same actions in response to verbal commands • E.g., confused open and close • In action-naming test, sometimes substituted nouns for verbs or omitted verbs: • “chairing” for sitting • “The man is a sack of potatoes” for a man carrying a sack of potatoes

  37. Verb deficit and agrammatism: Why? • Syntactic hypothesis • Verbs are by their nature syntactically complex • Nouns are not complex – they can stand alone • Semantic hypothesis – using proximity • Verbs represent processes and processes are managed by the frontal lobe • Nouns represent things, and things are known mainly through perception, which is managed by the occipital, temporal, and parietal lobes

  38. Noun-Verb vis-à-vis Speech & Writing(908b) • Patient S.J.D. • Written naming of verbs defective • But oral naming okay • Nouns okay for both writing and speaking • Patient H.W. • Oral naming of verbs defective • But written naming okay • Nouns okay for both writing and speaking • Comparable results independently of mode of stimulus – picture naming, reading, writing to dictation • (More, next slide..)

  39. More on H.W. & S.J.D.Noun-Verb vis-à-vis Speech-Writing(Rapp & Caramazza 908-9) • Inputs: pictures, oral dictation, reading • Tasks: (1) speaking, (2) writing • Example: • There’s a crack in the mirror (crack as n.) • Don’t crack the mirror (crack as v.) • S.J.D. • crack as n. correctly produced, both modalities • crack as v. correct only in spoken modality • H.W. – the opposite modality effect • Data fromCaramazza & Hillis (1991)

  40. Broca’s Area: A closer look

  41. Subdivisions of Broca’s area • Broca’s area includes two different (but adjacent) Brodmann areas • BA 44 – Pars Opercularis • BA 45 – Pars Triangularis • (Some people also include the Pars orbitalis, just inferior to the pars triangularis)

  42. Frontal Operculum • Operculum: little cover • The part of the frontal lobe that covers (part of) the Sylvian fissure and anterior insula • Adjacent to and inside the anterior portion of Sylvian fissure • Opposite it (across Sylvian fissure) in temporal lobe is the temporal operculum

  43. Subdivisions of Broca’s area • Another view

  44. Major Language Areas Supramarginal gyrus (Goldstein’s area) Angular gyrus (Geschwinds’s area) Wernicke’s area Broca’s area Superior parietal lobule Brodmann area 37

  45. Left hemi-sphere, showing middle cerebral artery Frontal Operculum

  46. A closer look at Broca’s aphasia • Broca’s original patient • Lesion was extensive • Not just Broca’s area but also • Adjacent areas • Subjacent white matter • A tradition has followed Broca • Broca’s area held responsible for symptoms of Broca’s aphasia • Confounding factor: • Broca’s area is only part of the area of damage with Broca’s aphasia

  47. More recent findings • Modern imaging methods help • Some patients have damage restricted mainly or entirely to just Broca’s area • They have less severe symptoms that typically associated with Broca’s aphasia • Influential paper: Alexander et al. 1990 • Examined Broca patients with different areas of damage • Cast doubts on importance of Broca’s area in Broca’s aphasia

  48. Broca’s area and Broca aphasia • Maybe it’s not just Broca’s area damage that is responsible for some of the symptoms of “Broca’s aphasia” • Maybe some of them result instead from damage to neighboring areas • Alexander et al. (1990) propose distinguishing 3 subtypes

  49. Three subtypes in Alexander study • Impaired speech initiation • Symptom traditionally attributed to transcortical motor aphasia • Area of damage: frontal operculum • Disturbed articulatory function • Area of damage: lower primary motor cortex • The classical Broca’s aphasia syndrome • More extensive damage

  50. To be continued … • (Later)

More Related