610 likes | 627 Views
Discover the definition and history of ergonomics and human factors, learn about Fitts' Law, and optimize workplace design for safety and efficiency. Explore the impact of ergonomics on human well-being and system performance.
E N D
Human Factors and Fitts’ Law Ken Goldberg, IEOR and EECS, UC Berkeley
What is Ergonomics? Prof. Wojciech Jastrzebowski in Poland in 1857: From two Greek words Ergon meaning work and Nomos meaning principles or laws Ergonomics = The Science of Work
What is Ergonomics? Common Definitions “Ergonomics is essentially fitting the workplace to the worker. The better the fit the higher the level of safety and worker efficiency.” Fitting the Task to the Human ~ Grandjean 1990 “Ergonomics removes barriers to quality, productivity and human performance by fitting products, tasks, and environments to people.” ErgoWeb.com
Human Factors What Is Human Factors? The following definition was adopted by the International Ergonomics Association in August 2000: Ergonomics (or human factors) is the scientific discipline concerned with the understanding of interactions among humans and other elements of a system, and the profession that applies theory, principles, data, and other methods to design in order to optimize human well-being and overall system performance.
Human Factors and Ergonomics • Britain - The Ergonomic Society was formed in 1952 with people from psychology, biology, physiology, and design. • United States - The Human Factors Society was formed in 1957. In the US "human factors engineering" was emphasized by the US military with concentration on human engineering and engineering psychology.
from Mike Mandel, Making Good Time (CMP Bulletin vol. 8 no. 2, California Museum of Photography, UC California, Riverside, 1989)
Hawthorne Effect Worker Study (1927 - 1932) of the Hawthorne Plant of the Western Electric Company in Cicero, Illinois. Led by Harvard Business School professor Elton Mayo: Effect of varying light levels on Productivity.
Occupational Safety and Health Administration, (OSHA, 1970, www.osha.gov)
Neutral Posture for Computer Use Position the monitor about an arm’s length away directly in front of you. The top of the screen no higher than eye level (Unless the user wears bi-focal glasses) Adjust the seat height so upper arms hang vertically, elbows bent about 90 degrees, shoulders relaxed and wrists fairly straight Use a document holder close to the monitor rather than laying papers flat Adjust the back rest to provide firm support to the small of the back Mouse should be next to keyboard both at a height equivalent to the user’s seated elbow height Knees comfortably bent with feet resting on the floor. If the chair is raised so the keyboard height equals elbow height, use a footrest .
Paul M. Fitts, 1954 Fitts connected the speed-accuracy tradeoff of choice reaction times to reaching movement tasks
A W ID A W Fitts’ “Law” • T = a + b log2( ) Parameters a, b experimentally determined
Alternative: Square-root Law • Fitts’ Logarithmic Law is not derived using biomechanics and kinematics • We derive a “Square-root” Law: based on 2 simple assumptions
Assumption 1 Acceleration ( ) is piecewise constant
Assumption 2 Acceleration is proportional to target width Wider targets are easier to reach larger accelerations possible
Optimal Control • Given a bound on , Fastest way to reach a target is to use “bang-bang” control T/2 T
Optimal Bang-Bang Control Velocity s = T/2 T Position at time T:
A A 2 Optimal Bang-Bang Control Position T s = T/2
Optimal Binary Acceleration Model • Use Assumption 2 to specify a single formula that relates A, W, and T • Assumption 2 Hypothesis: Maximal acceleration set by the human is proportional to target width (Wider targets permit larger accelerations)
Optimal Binary Acceleration Model • Assume: • Optimal bang-bang model: • Add reaction time a: • Parameters a,b set from experimental data
First Mouse Patent (Engelbart) (Shumin Zhai, IBM Almaden Research Center)
Experimental Tests Homogeneous Cursor Motions Heterogeneous Cursor Motions Fixed Rectangle Test Variable Rectangle Test Circle Test
Available Data • Original data set: • 2232 users for fixed rectangle tests • 2466 users for variable rectangle tests • 1897 users for circle test • User did not complete all trials Removed • User has outlier points Removed • Final data set: • 1640 users for fixed rectangle tests • 1996 users for variable rectangle tests • 1561 users for circle tests
Model Parameters • Parameter set using least-squares linear regression for each user • Average parameters over all users:
Effect Size • Mean signed difference in RMS errors between the Square-root Law and Fitts’ Logarithmic Law, as a percent of the mean RMS error for Fitts’ Logarithmic Law, with 95% confidence intervals Square-root Law better Logarithmic Law better
Web-Based Fitts’ Law Demo www.tele-actor.net/fitts/
Human Factors and Ergonomics • Britain - The Ergonomic Society was formed in 1952 • United States - The Human Factors Society was formed in 1957.
Human Factors and Fitts’ Law Ken Goldberg, IEOR and EECS, UC Berkeley
Outline • Fitts’ Law Introduction • Kinematics Models of Fitts’ Task • Symmetric Binary Acceleration Model • Asymmetric Binary Acceleration Model • Fitts’ Task in HCI • Web-based Experiments
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Choice Reaction Time Task Stimulus: 1,…,N Response: 1,…,N J. Merkel, 1885: Stimuli 1,…,N equally likely. TR = a + b log2N 4
Information Theory • Base 2 logarithm of the number of alternatives is a measure of information Number of bits = log2N Corresponds to the average number of yes/no questions required to identify correct stimulus • In example: log2 8 = 3 bits
Fitts’ Information Theory Approach • Define “information” encoded in a reaching moving task • Information transmitted I in a response is a measure of the reduction in uncertainty
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Information Transmitted 7-8 • # possibilities before event: 8 • # possibilities after event: 2 • Information transmitted: -log2(2/8) = 2 bits • Uncertainty: 1 bit 000 001 010 011 100 101 110 111
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Discrete vs. Continuous Choice 000 001 010 011 100 101 110 111 Target StartPosition Width W Amplitude A
Fitts’ Formulation Number of possibilities after response: W Number of possibilities before response: 2A Information transmitted = Index of Difficulty
Weber Fraction Formulation of Fitts’ Task • Welford, 1968 • Weber fraction: W/(A+0.5W) Target StartPosition Width W Amplitude A
Shannon Formulation of Fitts’ Task • Formulation based on Shannon’s Theorem[I. Scott MacKenzie 1992] • Shannon Formulation for Fitts’ Task: • C = Information capacity of communication channel • B = channel bandwidth • S = signal strength • N = noise power