200 likes | 833 Views
Early Controversies about Sensory and Perceptual Development. Nature vs. nurtureNativist philosophers argue that many basic perceptual abilities are innate.Empiricist philosophers believed infants are born tabula rasa (blank slate) and must learn to interpret sensations.Enrichment vs. differentiationEnrichment theory claims that sensory stimulation is often fragmented or confusing.Differentiation theory argues that sensory stimulation provides all we need to interpret our experiences..
E N D
1. Chapter 6 Early Cognitive Foundations: Sensation, Perception, and Learning
2. Early Controversies about Sensory and Perceptual Development Nature vs. nurture
Nativist philosophers argue that many basic perceptual abilities are innate.
Empiricist philosophers believed infants are born tabula rasa (blank slate) and must learn to interpret sensations.
Enrichment vs. differentiation
Enrichment theory claims that sensory stimulation is often fragmented or confusing.
Differentiation theory argues that sensory stimulation provides all we need to interpret our experiences.
3. "Making Sense" of the Infant's Sensory and Perceptual Experiences The preference method
Two stimuli are presented simultaneously to see whether infants will attend more to one of them than the other.
Robert Fantz's looking chamber
The habituation method
Most popular strategy for measuring infant sensory and perceptual capabilities
Infants habituate and dishabituate to many different kinds of stimulation
Evoked potentials: Brain wave patterns are studied.
High-amplitude sucking: Infants are provided with a special pacifier containing electrical circuitry that enables them to exert some control over the sensory environment.
4. Infant Sensory Capabilities Vision
Least developed sense in the newborn
Visual acuity of newborn: 20/600
Hearing
Reactions to voices: Especially attentive to high-pitched feminine voices
Reactions to language: Infants can discriminate phonemes very early in life.
Consequences of hearing loss: Could hamper language development
Taste and smell
Infants are born with preference for sweet tastes.
Infants are born capable of detecting a variety of odors.
Touch, temperature, and pain: Proprioceptor senses are functioning at birth.
5. Infant Sensory Capabilities (cont.)
6. Perception of Patterns and Forms in Infancy Early pattern perception (0 to 2 months)
Prefer to look at whatever they can see well
Later form perception (2 months to 1 year)
Can use object movement to perceive form
Explaining form perception
Growth of form perception results from a continuous interplay among baby's inborn equipment, biological maturation, and visual experiences.
7. Perception of Three-Dimensional Space in Infancy Size constancy
Recognizing that an object remains the same size even when its image on the retina becomes larger as the object moves closer, or smaller as the object moves farther away
Binocular vision enhances this capability.
Binocular vision emerges around 3 to 5 months of age.
Use of pictorial cues
7-month-olds seem able to use pictorial cues, while 5-month-olds cannot.
8. Perception of Three-Dimensional Space in Infancy (cont.) Development of depth perception
Visual cliff experiment
Infants detect a difference between the deep and shallow sides.
Motor development and depth perception
Self-produced movement helps develop depth perception.
9. Visual Cliff Visual cliff:
10. Perception of Three-Dimensional Space in Infancy (cont.)
11. Intermodal Perception The senses are integrated at birth or shortly thereafter.
Development of intermodal perception improves dramatically over the first year of life.
Explaining intermodal perception is difficult, but seems consistent with the differentiation theory.
12. Infant Perception in Perspective—and a Look Ahead Perceptual learning in childhood: Gibson’s differentiation theory
Perceptual learning occurs when we actively explore and detect distinctive features.
Cultural influences on perception
Subtle, but important effects
Newborns are equipped to perceive "musicality" and to discriminate good music from bad music.
Humans learn not to hear certain phonemes if they are not distinctive to the language spoken.
13. Infant Perception in Perspective—and a Look Ahead (cont.) Figure 6.14
Examples of figures used to test children’s ability to detect the distinctive features of letterlike
forms. Stimulus 1 is the standard. The child’s task is to examine each of the comparison stimuli
(stimuli 2–7) and pick out those that are the same as the standard. Adapted from
Gibson et al.,1962.
14. Basic Learning Processes Individual now thinks, perceives, or reacts to the environment in a new way
Result of a person's experiences
Change is relatively permanent.
15. Habituation: Early Evidence of Information-Processing and Memory Process by which we stop attending or responding to a stimulus repeated over and over
Improves dramatically throughout the first year of life
Individual differences
Infants who habituate rapidly during the first six to eight months of life are quicker to understand and use language during the second year of life.
16. Classical Conditioning A neutral stimulus that initially has no effect on the child eventually elicits a response of some sort, because it is associated with a second stimulus that always elicits the response.
Classical conditioning of emotions
Little Albert
UCS — loud banging noise
UCR — fearful behavior
CS — rat
CR— fearful behavior
Even newborns can be classically conditioned.
17. Classical Conditioning (cont.) Figure 6.15 The three phases of
classical conditioning. In the
preconditioning phase, the
unconditioned stimulus (UCS)
always elicits an unconditioned
response (UCR), whereas the
conditioned stimulus (CS) never
does. During the conditioning
phase, the CS and UCS are
paired repeatedly and eventually
associated. At this point, the
learner passes into the
postconditioning phase, in which
the CS alone elicits the original
response (now called a
conditioned response, or CR)
18. Operant (or Instrumental) Conditioning Four possible consequences of operant responses
Positive reinforcement: Something pleasant is added to increase response.
Negative reinforcement: Something unpleasant is removed to increase response.
Positive punishment: Something unpleasant is added to decrease response.
Negative punishment: Something pleasant is removed to decrease response.
Operant conditioning in infancy is at best limited in early infancy.
Infants can remember what they have learned.
The social significance of early operant learning is evident in infants and their caregivers.
19. Operant (or Instrumental) Conditioning (cont.) Figure 6.16
Basic principles of operant conditioning
20. Operant (or Instrumental) Conditioning (cont.)
21. Observational Learning Newborn imitation can be observed for facial expressions.
Advances in imitation and observational learning become obvious around 8 to 12 months of age.
Grade school children are capable of verbally describing model's behavior, and are better at imitating the model.