1 / 23

Joke City and beyond: supporting comprehension improvement through language, gesture and jokes

Joke City and beyond: supporting comprehension improvement through language, gesture and jokes. Nicola Yuill Psychology/Cognitive Science University of Sussex nicolay@sussex.ac.uk www.riddles.sussex.ac.uk. Plan of talk. Causes of poor comprehension Metalinguistic awareness

flora
Download Presentation

Joke City and beyond: supporting comprehension improvement through language, gesture and jokes

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Joke City and beyond: supporting comprehension improvement through language, gesture and jokes Nicola Yuill Psychology/Cognitive Science University of Sussex nicolay@sussex.ac.uk www.riddles.sussex.ac.uk

  2. Plan of talk • Causes of poor comprehension • Metalinguistic awareness • Aims of intervention • Joke City study • Language and comprehension gain • Gesture and comprehension gain • Conclusions and questions

  3. Poor comprehension 15% of 580 7-9yr olds had comprehension ages 6-24 mo below reading age Scores Vocab: choose 1 of 4 words to match a picture Neale Accuracy: read words in stories Neale Comprehension answer questions about the stories

  4. Poor reading comprehension: causes • Poor working memory: simultaneous storage and processing e.g. mental arithmetic • Poor inferential skill: John took 5 books. How many books? John pedalled over the bridge. How did John travel? • Poor ‘language awareness’: distinguishing form and meaning, knowing how you know (Yuill & Oakhill, 1992)

  5. Language awareness Treating text as interpreted: Poor comprehenders: • define reading in terms of decoding (best reader = read hardest words) • have difficulty with deductive inference: How do you know that x....? • have poor understanding of jokes that play on meaning (but not that play on sound)

  6. Not just a memory problem… Knowing how to use knowledge • Cain & Oakhill (1999) Stories: ‘They set off for home, pedalling as fast as they could.’ Questions: How did they travel home? Poor comprehenders 53% correct Prompts: If incorrect: • look at story again -> 68% • E directs child to relevant part of text ->85% • Clue: What sort of things can we pedal? ->100%

  7. Ambiguity resolution problems 48 children, 7-9yrs, varying in comprehension skill: ‘Look at that bat’ (choose 2 of 4 pictures) ‘The man said the duck was ready to eat.’ What could it mean? What else could it mean? (TLC, Wiig, 1998) Comprehension skill predicts ambiguity score, r(46) = .46, p<.001 (w/o acc and age)

  8. Understanding communication Ambiguous message game (Robinsons, 1978) X tells Y: ‘Pick the man with the flag’ Did X tell Y properly? What should X have said? Comprehension skill predicts message judgement score r(33) = .49, p<.001 (w/o accuracy and age) Children with comprehension age under 8;5 scored 6/8 or less

  9. Jokes (BAHLAS:Yuill & George, 2000) 50 jokes playing on meanings: choose correct punchline Why did the leopard never escape from the zoo? Because it was always spotted/Because it ran too slowly Tested on 300 children yrs 3-6 Good reliability (.83) and good prediction of comprehension independently of accuracy (r over .6) (No relation of comprehension and jokes playing on sound: What room can’t you go into? A mushroom) BAHLAS Riddles scores

  10. Language awareness and comprehension • Poor sensitivity to meaning • Poor understanding of communication • Poor understanding of meaning ambiguity • Poor understanding of jokes

  11. Aims of intervention: Questions • Practice: benefits to children, esp. given the neglect of comprehension • Process: theoretical understanding of comprehension processes. Children lack x, train x, does comprehension improve? • Relation of intervention to normal comprehension processes: what should the control conditions be? Why don’t some children develop x naturally? Is x a piece of knowledge or an attitude to reading?

  12. Comprehension training • Some training is very reflective, teaching explicit and metacognitive knowledge and strategies (e.g. Paris) • Some work assumes implicit (not directly trainable?) processes (e.g. Gernsbacher, inhibition) • MLA seems amenable to training to improve comprehension • By encouraging children to have an interrogative attitude to text –how? Make it problematic…

  13. MLA training can work: • 1 session of training children to search for ‘clue’ words in deliberately ambiguous texts brings significant comprehension increase on similar texts. (Yuill & Joscelyne 1988) • 7 30-minute group sessions, riddle training vs ‘funny stories’. Significant comprehension increase for riddle group on standardised test. (Yuill, 1998) • 3 25-minute sessions with Joke City software: explaining jokes, requiring children to articulate ambiguity and alternate interpretations of text, sig. better on Neale compr. than control no treatment. (Yuill & Bradwell, 1998)

  14. Comprehension skills improve after discussing ambiguity in joking riddles • 12 pairs of 7- to 9-year-old children (same sex, one good + one poor) • 3 sessions Joke City software • 24 control children (no treatment) • Transcribed videos of sessions 1 and 3 of 3 x 25-minute sessions for 12 pairs • Standardised comprehension test pre- and post-training (2 parallel forms of Neale) pre- to post-training changes (months) in accuracy and comprehension scores Individual differences!

  15. Looking at process: Joke City in more detail • Do children whose comprehension improves talk about different things from children who don’t improve? • Does what children talk about change across the training sessions? • Do any changes across sessions relate to how much children improve in comprehension? (so is metalinguistic awareness an engine for change?) • Does this restaurant serve fish? • Yes, what would you like to eat, Mr Fish?

  16. Coding scheme (Yuill & George 2006) • Metacognitive: self, other or joint knowledge or ignorance, thinking aloud Aah, I get it! I don’t understand. Do you know? We did it right. • Metalinguistic: defining cued or uncued meaning, or both, metalinguistic play and exploration Does this restaurant serve fish?’ –‘Yes, what do you want to eat, Mr Fish?’ ‘serve’: cued meaning = object which is served, uncued meaning = agent to whom food is served. Cued AND uncued at once: I get it! Cos they serve fish on a plate and they serve fish to the fish. • Control: task management, responses to control: Your turn to read • Reading from screen Does this restaurant serve fish? Reliability over 90%

  17. High improvers made more metalinguistic comments than medium or low improvers in Session 3 Interaction of group x session, p<.05. Mean no. of utterances combining cued and uncued meanings in s3: Hi: 1.25, med: 1.0, lo: 0.13 (lo) Comprehension change and metalinguistic comments r (21) = .49, p<.02 Low improvers made more metacognitive comments Number of utterances in each talk category Sig. interaction group x talk category, p<.001, mcog higher for low than med or hi

  18. Examples of cued/uncued comments • spotted ‘cos leopards they have spots and it’s cos they get spotted’ (= seen) • serve fish ‘he’s a fish and he likes to eat fish’ • bed socks ‘you wear them to bed and the bed’s wearing them’ • pinch ‘you can pinch someone on the leg or you can pinch sweets without paying’ • roll ‘you can roll a sausage roll’ Hard to articulate…. Other ways to express?

  19. What about gestures? • Gestures seem to indicate concepts on the brink of a child’s understanding (Goldin-Meadow, Pine) • Mismatches: children who express one idea in speech and another in gesture are more likely to improve on balance beam task than gesture-speech matchers (Pine et al., 2004) • Gestures show the listener what the actor understands • Perhaps gestures help a child’s own understanding • Language ambiguity seems to be a prime example where gestures might be used before speech

  20. Method of coding gestures for Joke City • Collected all clips containing gestures (N=100) • Gestures coded as referring to cued (obvious) or uncued (non-obvious) meaning of the joke: • How do you make a sausage roll? Push it down a hill?’ roll = ‘pastry’ = cued, ‘rotate’ = uncued • Usually clear: adults can recognise which meaning, without sound • Number of cued and uncued gestures (counting repeated gestures once only)

  21. Gestures: examples

  22. Associations of gestures and comprehension measures • Tot. no. gestures and pre-test reading comprehension r(20) = .61 cued, .58 uncued (r accuracy both <.2, n.s., vocab r .2-.4, n.s., semantic fluency n.s.) • Gestures and ambiguous word test, r(20) = .51 (cued), .61 (uncued) • Gestures and metalinguistic utterances No relation of gestures to other talk categories, or to improvement

  23. Book plug… .

More Related