200 likes | 305 Views
Pitch Contour Following Response (PCFR) in Infants. By: Elizabeth Schnabel Advised By: Dr. Jeng. Introduction. Voice pitch is an important auditory perception that reflects the brain’s ability to discriminate the pitch contour of speech signals. FFR – Frequency Following Response.
E N D
Pitch Contour Following Response (PCFR) in Infants By: Elizabeth Schnabel Advised By: Dr. Jeng
Introduction • Voice pitch is an important auditory perception that reflects the brain’s ability to discriminate the pitch contour of speech signals.
FFR – Frequency Following Response • Evoked neural response from the brainstem which is phase locked to the periodicity/pitch of the stimulus harmonics. • Especially the Fundamental Frequency • Series of sinusoidal peaks – intervals between peaks reveals the pitch information of the stimulus (Moushegian et al., 1973) Frequency=1/sec =.002 sec 1/.002 =500Hz
Recent Studies Krishnan et al. 2004, Krishnan et al. 2005, and Swaminathan et al. 2008. Investigated: FFR’s ability to follow a harmonic pitch contour in a stimulus - Pitch Contour Following Response (PCFR) Major findings: • It was feasible to record PCFR in adults. • Native speakers of Mandarin have greater pitch strength and more accurate pitch tracking • yi2 and yi3 have greater pitch strength Krishnan et al. (2004) Time (ms)
Purpose • Further define the characteristics of the PCFR and its developmental trajectory • Facilitate a better understanding of how infant discriminate pitch changes during the critical period of language acquisition.
Questions (1) What is the feasibility of recording PCFR from individual infants? (2) How does age relate to the PCFR response? (3) How does infant response compare to adult response?
Methods - Participants • 9 Adults • Age range: 23-28 years • Mean ± SD = 24.2 ± 2.0 years • 7 Infants • Age range: 1-11 months • Mean ± SD = 4.86 ± 3.48 months Methods - Stimulus • Voiced Mandarin Chinese syllable • Rising lexical pitch contour: /yi/ meaning ‘aunt’.
Methods- Protocol • One session • Consent form reviewed and signed • Normal hearing established • Adult: Air Conduction <20dB between 250-8000Hz • Infant: Distortion Product Otoacoustic Emissions - 2000-8000 Hz. • Electrode/ear tip placement • State of restfulness was achieved • Stimulus and recording started • Two trials of 1200 tokens • Two trials of 1200 control
Results • Spectrograms • Pitch Strength - Amplitude • Latency
Results • Typical Spectrograms • Show spectral energy bands which can reflect harmonic pitch change over time.
Results • Spectrograms of all Infants
Discussion - Spectrogram • Visual representation of the infants ability to follow pitch contour over time. • The feasibility of recording PCFR in individual infants as early as one month of age
Results • Pitch Strength - Autocorrelation - correlating the response to itself • Estimation of pitch period • Estimation of response pitch strength • peak to trough amplitude Stimulus
Results • Amplitude: Pitch strength
Discussion – Pitch Strength • Consistent with Gardi et al. (1979) - Compared tone-burst elicited FFR in 22 neonates (<3 days old) and the average adults responses from his doctoral dissertation. • line of best fit suggests that the neonates had a larger amplitude at 55dBnHL compared to adults at 250 Hz tone burst
Discussion – Pitch Strength cont. • Larger amplitude could also be contributed by: • Infants smaller head size • Infant small ear canal volume
Results • Latency - Cross correlation between the stimulus and recording
Discussion - Latency • Gardi et al. (1979) • Consistant – Longer latencies as frequency decreases • Difference – Gardi et al. had a significant difference between infant and adult latencies at low frequencies. p=0.0813
Conclusion (1) It is feasible to record a PCFR in individual infantsas early as one month of age. (2) Further data is needed to be obtained from more infants to better delineate the developmental trajectory of infant PCFR. (3) There is significant difference between the mean of infant and adult pitch strength amplitude.
References • Aiken, S.J., Picton, T.W. (2006). Envelope following responses to natural vowels. Audiology & Neuro-Otology, 11(4), 213-232. • American Electroencephalographic Society. (1994). Guideline seven: a proposal for standard montages to be used in clinical EEG. J Clin Neurophysiol, 11(1), 30-36. • Clarkson, M., Clifton, R., (1985). Infant pitch perception: Evidence for responding to pitch categories and the missing fundamental. J. Acoustic. Soc. Am., 77, 1521-1528. • Dajani, H.R., Purcell, D., Wong, W., Kunov, H., Picton, T.W. (2005). Recording human evoked potentials that follow the pitch contour of a natural vowel. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 52(9), 1614-1618. • Galbraith, G.C., Philippart, M., Stephen, L.M. (1996). Brainstem frequency-following responses in Rett syndrome. Pediatric Neurology, 15(1), 26-31. • Gardi, J., Salamy, A., Mendelson, T. (1979). Scalp-recorded frequency-following responses in neonates. Audiology: Journal of Auditory Communication, 18(6), 494-506. • Greenberg, S., Marsh, J.T., Brown, W.S., Smith, J.C. (1987). Neural temporal coding of low pitch. I. Human frequency following responses to complex tone. Hearing Research, 25, 91-114. • Hu, J., Jeng, F.-C. (2009). An automatic algorithm for detection human frequency following response to voice pitch. In: Abstracts of American Auditory Society Annual Meeting, Poster #31, Scottsdale, Arizona, March 5-7. • John, M.S., Brown, D.K., Muir, P.J., Picton, T.W. (2004) Recording auditory steady-state responses in young infants. Ear and Hearing, 25(6), 539-553. • Krishnan, A., Parkinson, J. (1999). Human frequency-following responses to two-tone approximations of steady-state vowels. Audiol Neurootol, 4, 95-103. • Krishnan, A. (2002). Human frequency-following responses: representation of steady-state synthetic vowels. Hearing Research, 166, 192-201. • Krishnan, A., Xu, Y., Gandour, J., Cariani, P. (2004). Human frequency-following response: Representation of pitch contours in Chinese tones. Hearing Research,189,1-12. • Krishnan, A., Xu, Y., Gandour, J., Cariani, P. (2005). Encoding of pitch in the human brainstem is sensitive to language experience. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 161-168. • Levi, E.C., Folsom, R.C., Dobie, R.A. (1995). Coherence analysis of envelope-following responses (EFRs) and frequency-following responses (FFRs) in infants and adults. Hearing Research, 89(1-2), 21-27. • Moushegian, G., Rupert, A.L., Stillman, R.D. (1973). Laboratory note. Scalp-recorded early responses in man to frequencies in the speech range. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 35(6), 665-667. • Orfanidis, S. J. (1996). Optimum signal processing: An introduction. Englewood, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. • Salamy, A. (1984). Maturation of the auditory brainstem response from birth through early childhood. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 1(3), 293-329. • Savio, G., Cárdenas, J., Pérez Abalo, M., González, A., Valdés, J. (2001). The low and high frequency auditory steady state responses mature at different rates. Audiology & Neuro-Otology, 6(5), 279-287. • Swaminathan, J., Krishnan, A., Gandour, J.T. (2008b). Pitch encoding in speech and nonspeech contexts in the human auditory brainstem. NeuroReport, 19(11), 1163-1167. • Swaminathan, J., Krishnan, A., Gandour, J.T., Xu, Y. (2008a). Applications of static and dynamic iterated rippled noise to evaluate pitch encoding in the human auditory brainstem. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 55(1), 281-287. • Worden, F.G., March J.T. (1968). Frequency-following (microphonic-like) neural responces evoked by sound. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol, 25, 42-52. • Yamada, O., Marsh, R.R., Handler, S.D. (1982). Contributing generator of frequency-following response in man. Scandinavian Audiology, 11(1), 53-56. • Young, E.D., Sachs, M.B. (1979). Representation of steady-state vowels in the temporal aspects of the discharge patterns of populations of auditory-nerve fibers. J Acoustic Soc Am, 66, 1381-1403.