1 / 36

The Chomsky Hierarchy and the Complexity of the English Language

The Chomsky Hierarchy and the Complexity of the English Language. Jamie Frost – Franks Society MT10. What is language?. “a body of words and the systems for their use common to a people who are of the same community or nation, the same geographical area, or the same cultural tradition”.

harvey
Download Presentation

The Chomsky Hierarchy and the Complexity of the English Language

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. The Chomsky Hierarchy and the Complexity of the English Language Jamie Frost – Franks Society MT10

  2. What is language? “a body of words and the systems for their use common to a people who are of the same community or nation, the same geographical area, or the same cultural tradition” “the system of linguistic signs or symbols considered in the abstract (opposed to speech)” “the specifically human capacity for acquiring and using complex systems of communication”

  3. What is a language? OED “The system of spoken or written communication used by a particular country, people, community, etc., typically consisting of words used within a regular grammatical and syntactic structure.” Wikipedia “A set of symbols of communication and the elements used to manipulate them.”

  4. What is a language • An alphabet Σ The possible symbols that each ‘unit’ in the language can take. For human languages, the alphabet may be at a character level. Σ = { a, b, c, ... , y, z, ?, !, &, ‘SPACE’, ... } Or we could choose it to be at a word level... Σ = { A, AARDVARK, ... , ZEBRA }

  5. What is a language • An alphabetΣ • ...can form a ‘string’ Σ* Σ2 = Σ × Σ gives us all the possible pairs of symbols. Σ2 = { A A, A AARDVARK, ... , ZEBRA ZEBRA } Σ* = {λ} ∪Σ ∪ (Σ × Σ) ∪ (Σ × Σ× Σ) ∪ ... is known as the Kleene Star, and gives us all possible strings, i.e. containing any combination of symbols and of any length. Σ* = { A THE LOBSTER FRENCH TULIP, Who’S THERE? NAY ANSWER ME ... , PILLOW, ... }

  6. What is a language • An alphabet Σ • ...can form a ‘string’Σ* • ...which can be constrained by a grammar Any sensible language doesn’t allow a unrestricted combination of symbols. Human languages are bounded by some grammatical structure. ENGLISH ⊆ Σ* ENGLISH = { That’s a rather nice top you’re wearing, That tulip stole my monkey, what’s that coming over the hill - is it a monster? , .... }

  7. Alphabet + Grammar = LANGUAGE Mint.

  8. Grammars • So how do we define a grammar? • Grammars limit our possible strings to certain forms. • These vary in expressiveness – the more expressible they are, the harder it is to do certain common tasks with them. • Tasks might include: • “finding the grammatical structure of a string given a grammar”, or • “does a string satisfy a given grammar?”. • “are two grammars equivalent?” Expressiveness Task Complexity

  9. The Chomsky Hierarchy • In 1956, Noam Chomsky characterised languages according to how ‘complex’ or expressible they are. • This is known as the Chomsky Hierarchy. • A language that satisfies a given type is also a instance of all the grammars above it. Complexity

  10. A Formal Grammar • Consists of: Terminal symbols (i.e. our alphabet Σ) A start symbol (S ϵ N) S⟶i love T T⟶T and T T⟶smurfs T⟶smurfettes Non-terminal symbols (N) Production rules

  11. A Formal Grammar Think of it as a game... Grammar A possible derived string S ⟶ i love T ⟶ i love T and T ⟶ i love smurfs and T ⟶ i love smurfs and T and T ⟶ i love smurfs and smurfs and T ⟶ i love smurfs and smurfs and smurfettes B) We can use the production rules the replace things. S⟶i love T T⟶T and T T⟶smurfs T⟶smurfettes A) Start with start symbol C) We’re not allowed to ‘finish’ until we only have terminal symbols.

  12. Regular Grammars • The most restrictive. • The LHS of the production rules can only be a single non-terminal. • The RHS of the production rules can be one of (a) a single terminal symbol (b) a single non-terminal symbol (c) a terminal followed by a non-terminal or (d) the empty symbol. The idea is that you don’t have ‘memory’ of the symbols you’ve previously emitted in the string. • B ⟶ a • B ⟶ A • B ⟶ aA • B ⟶ ϵ

  13. Regular Grammars • Example Example Generation: S ⟶ aS ⟶ aaS ⟶ aaaT ⟶ aaabT ⟶aaabb { ambn | m,n≥1 } S ⟶ aS S ⟶ aT T ⟶ bT T ⟶ b Notice we’re always generating at the end of the string.

  14. Regular Grammar • Harder Example: If Σ = {a,b}, the language where two b’s are never seen next to each other. • e.g. a, ababa, baa, b, aaa, ... • As a regular expression: L = a*(baa*)*(b|ϵ) • Often helpful to draw a picture: a This kind of diagram is known as a ‘nondeterministic finite automaton’ or NFA. b S T a

  15. Regular Grammar • We can use this picture to work out the regular grammar: S ⟶ ϵ S ⟶ aS S ⟶ bT T ⟶ ϵ T ⟶ aS a b S T a

  16. It’s Voting Time... Which are regular? The language of palindromes, i.e. strings which are the same when reversed, e.g. “madam”, “acrobats stab orca”. { anbn | n ≥ 1 } i.e. ab, aabb, aaabbb, aaaabbbb, ... Neither are. The problem is that we cannot ‘remember’ the symbols already emitted. We can use something called the pumping lemma to check if a language is regular.

  17. Context Free Grammars CFG to the rescue! MAN • The restriction on the RHS of the production rules is now loosened; we can have any combination of non-terminals and terminals. • We still restrict the LHS however to a single non-terminal. This is why the grammar is known as “context free”, since the production is not dependent on the context in which it occurs. While generating a string: ... ⟶ abXd ⟶ abyd The production rule which allows the X to become a y is not contingent on the context, i.e. The preceding b or the proceeding d.

  18. Context Free Grammars • Examples Example Generation: S ⟶ aSa ⟶ acSca ⟶ acbca Palindromes S ⟶ ϵ S ⟶ a S ⟶ b S ⟶ c S ⟶ aSa S ⟶ bSb S ⟶ cSc

  19. Context Free Grammars • Examples Example generation: S ⟶ aSb ⟶ aaSbb ⟶ aaaSbbb ⟶ aaabbb anbn S ⟶ ϵ S ⟶ aSb

  20. It’s Voting Time... Is this a CFG? { anbncn| n ≥ 1 } i.e. abc, aabbcc, aaabbbccc, ... Nope. A bit harder to see this time. Can use a variant of the Pumping Lemma called the Bar-Hillel Lemma to show it isn’t. (But informal explanation as such: We could have non-terminals at the a-b and b-c boundary generating these pairs, but since our language is context free these non-terminals expand independently of each other, thus we can only ensure a and b have the same count, or b and c. And we can’t have a rule of the form S-> X abc Y because then we can’t subsequently increase the number of b’s.)

  21. Context-Sensitive Grammars • Now an expansion of a non-terminal is dependent on the context it appears in. Example generation: S ⟶ aSBC ⟶ aaBCBC ⟶ aaBHBC ⟶ aaBBCC ⟶ aabBCC ⟶ aabbCC ⟶ aabbcC ⟶ aabbcc anbncn S ⟶ aSBC S ⟶ aBC CB ⟶ HB HB ⟶ BC aB ⟶ ab bB ⟶ bb bC ⟶ bc cC ⟶ cc i.e. a ‘C’ can change into a ‘c’ only when preceded by another ‘c’. Note that this context (i.e. this preceding ‘c’) must remain unchanged. Preservation of context is the only restriction in CSGs.

  22. The Chomsky Hierarchy Complexity The picture with circles and arrows we saw earlier.

  23. English as a CFG • Before we get on to classifying English according to the Chomsky Hierarchy, let’s see how English might be represented as a CFG. 1. Starting non-terminal Our starting non-terminal S is a sentence. Since sentences operate independently syntactically, it’s sufficient to examine grammar on a sentence level. 2. Alphabet Our terminals/alphabet Σ is just a dictionary in the literal sense. Σ = { a, aardvark, .... , zebra, zoology, zyzzyva }

  24. English as a CFG 3. Non-Terminals Our non-terminals are ‘constituents’, such as noun phrases, verb phrases, verbs, determiners, prepositional phrases, etc. These can be subdivided into further constituents (e.g. NP = noun phrase), or generate a terminals (e.g. V = verb)

  25. English as a CFG 4. Production Rules Can use an American style ‘top-down’ generative form of grammar. S ⟶ NP VP NP ⟶ DT N NP ⟶ PN VP ⟶ VP PP NP ⟶ NP PP PP ⟶ P NP NP ⟶ NP CONJ NP DT ⟶ the DT ⟶ a N ⟶ monkey N ⟶ student N ⟶ telescope PN ⟶ Corey P ⟶ with P ⟶ over CONJ ⟶ and CONJ ⟶ or V ⟶ saw V ⟶ ate

  26. Ambiguity • Curiously, it’s possible to generate a sentence in multiple ways! S ⟶ NP VP⟶ PN VP ⟶ Corey VP ⟶ Corey V NP ⟶ Corey saw NP ⟶ Corey saw NP PP ⟶ ... ⟶ Corey saw the monkey with the telescope. S ⟶ NP VP ⟶ PN VP ⟶ Corey VP ⟶ Corey VP PP ⟶ Corey V NP PP ⟶ Corey saw NP PP ⟶ ... ⟶ Corey saw the monkey with the telescope. “Audience Interaction Moment”: What do each of these sentences semantically mean?

  27. Ambiguity We say that a formal grammar that can yield the same string from multiple derivations is ‘ambiguous’.

  28. So what kind of language... is English? • Is a Regular Grammar sufficient? • Nope! • Chomsky demonstrates this by showing a particular part of English grammar that behaves akin to a palindrome. We saw earlier that palindromes are not regular. • But there’s a nicer more recent proof...

  29. Embedded Structures (Yngve 60) The cat likes tuna fish. The cat the dog chased likes tuna fish. The cat the dog the rat bit chased likes tuna fish. The cat the dog the rat the elephant admired bit chased likes tuna fish.

  30. Embedded Structures The cat the dog the rat the elephant admired bit chased likes tuna fish. 3 nouns 3 verbs If we let A = { the dog, the rat, the elephant } and B = { admired, bit, chased } then we represent centre-embedding as such: L = The cat an bnlikes tuna fish, a ϵ A, b ϵ B But we already know from earlier that anbnis not regular!

  31. So what kind of language... is English? • Is a Context Free Grammar sufficient? • Probably... • The fact constituents in a sentence are syntactically independent of each other naturally supports a grammar that is ‘context free’. • A number of scholars initially argued that number agreement (e.g. “Which problem did your professor say was unsolvable?”) couldn’t be captured by a CFG, but they were just being a bit dim. This can easily be incorporated by just introducing a few new non-terminals (e.g. NP_PLUR, NP_SING). • But then are all human languages context-free?

  32. Swiss German • A number of languages, such as Dutch and Swiss German, allow for cross-serial dependencies English: “...we have wanted to let the children help Hans paint the house.” ...merd’chindem Hans eshuushaendwelelaahalfeaastriiche ...we the children/ACC Hans/DAT the house/ACC have wanted to let help paint. DAT = Dative noun: the indirect object of a verb (e.g. John gave Mary the book”). ACC = Accusative noun: the direct object of a verb (e.g. John gave Mary the book”).

  33. Swiss German Shieber (1985) notes that among such sentences, those with all accusative NPs preceding all dative NPs, and all accusative-subcategorising verbs preceding all dative-subcategorising verbs are acceptable. English: “...we have wanted to let the children help Hans paint the house.” ...merd’chindem Hans eshuushaendwelelaahalfeaastriiche ...we the children/ACC Hans/DAT the house/ACC have wanted to let help paint. The number of verbs requiring dative objects (halfe) must equal the number of dative NPs (em Hans) and similarly for accusatives. Can therefore put in form: “Jan sait das meran bmeshuushaendwelecn dmaastriiche.”

  34. Swiss German English: “...we have wanted to let the children help Hans paint the house.” ...merd’chindem Hans eshuushaendwelelaahalfeaastriiche ...we the children/ACC Hans/DAT the house/ACC have wanted to let help paint. anbmcndm is not context free. It’s context-sensitive. Therefore Swiss German is not context free.

  35. Summary • The Chomsky Hierarchy brings together different grammar formalisms, listing them in increasing order of expressiveness. • Languages don’t have to be human ones: they allow us to generate strings with some given alphabet Σ, subject to some grammatical constraints. • The least expressive languages are Regular Grammars, which are insufficient to represent the English language. • But Context Free Grammars are insufficient to represent languages with cross-serial dependencies, such as Swiss German.

  36. Thanks! Questions?

More Related