1 / 1

Participants 15 adults from the Pittsburgh community 18-33 yrs; M = 24.4, SD = 5.3; 6 females

Motor. Behavioral Results. Method. Parietal. Verbal Protocol Results. Codes. PF: Encode proof statement GV: Encode givens IC: Intermediate conclusion FC : Final conclusion. BD, CD. BDCD. These two the same, And angle the same, ok, it is congruent, three step.

holly
Download Presentation

Participants 15 adults from the Pittsburgh community 18-33 yrs; M = 24.4, SD = 5.3; 6 females

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Motor Behavioral Results Method Parietal Verbal Protocol Results Codes PF: Encode proof statement GV: Encode givens IC: Intermediate conclusion FC: Final conclusion BD, CD. BDCD. These two the same, And angle the same, ok, it is congruent, three step. Difficulty:p < .0005 Difficulty:p < .0005 5x + 3 = 38 Outside World Visual Perception Type x=7 Manual Control Parse 5x+3=38 Prefrontal Anterior Cingulate Hold 5x=35 Production System Declarative Memory (Retrieval) Imaginal (ProblemState) Retrieve 38-3=35 Goal State “Unwinding” “Retrieving” Caudate What are they thinking? An Information Processing Approach to Deconstructing a Complex Task • • • •• • • Yvonne Kao (ykao@andrew.cmu.edu) and John Anderson (ja+@cmu.edu), Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University fMRI • Background • Goals • To use a variety of converging methods to understand the underlying cognitive processes that support geometry proof. • Verbal protocol analysis • Eyetracking • Neuroimaging • Cognitive modeling • To use our cognitive model to design an educational intervention to improve teaching and learning of geometry in the United States. • Motivation • Geometry and geometry proof are important • “Geometric thinking is an absolute necessity in every branch of mathematics.”(Cuoco, Goldenberg, & Mark, 1996, p.389) • Formal proof is an important part of mathematical discovery and problem-solving.(Schoenfeld, 1992) • Geometry and geometry proof are difficult for American students.(Martin & McCrone, 2001) • Geometry needs to become a more visible part of the middle grades curriculum. Too often it is ignored until high school, and delays students’ experience in a highly valuable and applicable part of mathematics. Geometric thinking and spatial visualization are linked to many other areas of mathematics, such as algebra, fractions, data, and chance. Teachers should spend more time developing geometric concepts (concretely and with many different representations) and principles (in varied settings) and not merely focus on practice involving algorithmic properties. (Wilson & Blank, 1999, p.13) • ACT-R • ACT-R is a cognitive architecture that defines specific cognitive processes and maps them to specific regions in the brain. ACT-R can provide a framework for predicting, interpreting, fitting, and discussing behavioral and neuroimaging data (Anderson, Bothell, Byrne, Douglass, Lebiere, & Qin, 2004). Participants 15 adults from the Pittsburgh community18-33 yrs; M = 24.4, SD = 5.3; 6 females Design 2 x 3 within-subjects full factorial design Task Determine if the minimum number of logical inferences needed to complete the proof is 1 inference, 3 inferences, or if the proof is not provable. The goal statement is marked at the bottom of the diagram. • Procedure • Geometry Review • Participants were given a self-paced review of basic plane geometry theorems and concepts: • Reflexivity • Vertical angles • Parallel lines • Isosceles triangles • Triangle congruence • Task Training • Participants were trained to a minimum of 75% accuracy on the geometry proof task. • fMRI Scanning • Participants performed 120 proofs while being scanned in a fMRI machine (TR = 2 seconds). • Verbal Protocol • 6 participants returned for an additional verbal protocol and eyetracking session. • Model • Goal stage (2.1 seconds) – encode the statement to be proven • Inference stage (2.1, 6.3, or 10.5 seconds) – integrate givens and intermediate inferences to formulate the proof • Decision stage (2.1 seconds) – make the response • Post-decision reflection (time not modeled) Conclusions Participants reliably encode the goal statement at the beginning of each proof. This is counter to findings from Koedinger & Anderson’s (1990) study of expert geometry problem solvers. This could be due to the different nature of the task, or the different populations from which the participants were drawn. The critical process to understand appears to be how proficient problem-solvers integrate problem givens and diagram information to support their logical inferences, and how this process differs in experts, proficient problem solvers, and novices. References Anderson, J. R., Bothell, D., Byrne, M. D., Douglass, S., Lebiere, C., & Qin. Y. (2004). An integrated theory of mind. Psychological Review, 111(4), 1036-1060. Cuoco, A., Goldenberg, E. P., & Mark, J. (1996). Habits of mind: An organizing principle for mathematics curricula. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 15(4), 375-402. Koedinger, K. R. & Anderson, J. R. (1990). Abstract planning and perceptual chunks: Elements of expertise in geometry. Cognitive Science, 14, 511-550. Martin, T. S. & McCrone, S. S. (2001). Investigating the teaching and learning of proof: First year results. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, 585-94. Schoenfeld, A. H. (1992). Learning to think mathematically: Problem solving, metacognition, and sense-making in mathematics. In D. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook for Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning (pp.334-370). New York: MacMillan. Wilson, L. D. & Blank, R. K. (1999). Improving mathematics education using results from NAEP and TIMSS (ISBN-1-884037-56-9). Washington, DC: Council of Chief State School Officers. Ongoing and Future Work Analyze the eye movement data. Re-run this study with the portion of the population that couldn’t be trained to proficiency on the task in the time provided and compare the results.

More Related