1 / 17

Experimental design 2:

Experimental design 2:. Good experimental designs have high internal validity: To unequivocally establish causality, we need to ensure that groups in our study differ systematically only on our intended independent variable(s) and not on other confounding variables as well.

jmarriott
Download Presentation

Experimental design 2:

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Experimental design 2:

  2. Good experimental designs have high internal validity: To unequivocally establish causality, we need to ensure that groups in our study differ systematically only on our intended independent variable(s) and not on other confounding variables as well.

  3. Threats to the internal validity of an experiment's results (e.g. Campbell and Stanley 1969): Time threats: History Maturation Selection-maturation interaction Repeated testing Instrument change Group threats: Initial non-equivalence of groups Regression to the mean Differential mortality Control group awareness of its status. Participant reactivity threats: Experimenter effects, reactivity, evaluation apprehension.

  4. Types of experimental design: 1. Quasi-experimental designs: No control over allocation of subjects to groups, or timing of manipulations of the independent variable. (a) “One-group post-test" design: Prone to time effects, and no baseline against which to measure effects - pretty useless!

  5. (b) One group pre-test/post test design: Now have a baseline against which to measure effects of treatment. Still prone to time effects. Statistics marks 2006 Statistics marks 2007 course change

  6. (c) Interrupted time-series design: measurement measurement time measurement treatment measurement measurement measurement Still prone to time effects.

  7. (c) Interrupted time-series design (cont.): Deaths for Friday nights, 10-12 pm; Saturday and Sunday nights, 10 pm - 4 am. Vertical line: implementation of British Road Safety Act, Oct. 1967 (Ross, Campbell & Glass, 1970).

  8. (c) “Static group comparison" design: group A: measurement treatment (experimental gp.) group B: measurement no treatment (control gp.) Subjects are not allocated randomly to groups; therefore observed differences may be due to pre-existing group differences.

  9. 2. True experimental designs: (a) Post-test only/control group" design: group A: treatme nt measurement (experimental random gp.) allocation: group B: measurement no treatment (control gp.) Random allocation of subjects to groups should ensure that observed differences are not due to pre-existing group differences - but can't be certain!

  10. (b) Pre-test / post-test control group" design: measurement gro up measurem ent treatment A: random allocation: group measurement no treatment measurement B: Ensures that groups are indeed comparable before the experimental manipulation was administered.

  11. (c) Solomon four group design: measurement treatment measurement group A: measurement no treatment measurement group B: random allocation: treatment measurement group C: no treatment group measurement D: Ensures that groups are indeed comparable before the experimental manipulation was administered, and that pre-testing hasn't affected performance. (Uses lots of subjects, so rarely used).

  12. Between-groups versus within-subjects designs: Between-groups (independent measures) - Each subject participates in only one condition of the study. e.g. sex differences in memory. Within-subjects (repeated measures) - Each subject does all of the conditions in a study. e.g. effects of alcohol on memory. Mixed designs - Mixture of both. e.g, sex differences in effects of alcohol on memory.

  13. Advantages and disadvantages of between-groups and within-subjects designs:

  14. Within-subjects designs and order effects: Order effects: practice, fatigue, boredom. A fixed order of conditions would cause order to vary systematically with condition - results are uninterpretable, because they could be due to order effects, experimental manipulations or both. Solutions: (a) Randomise order of conditions: e.g. with 3 conditions, subjects randomly get orders ABC, BCA, ACB, CBA, CAB, BAC. (b) Counterbalance order of conditions: e.g. equal numbers of subjects get each order.

  15. A simple within-subjects design: subject 2: time treatment A measurement A subject 1: treatment B treatment B measurement B measurement A treatment A measurement A

  16. Disadvantages of the experimental method: Intrusive - participants know they are being observed, and this may affect their behaviour. Experimenter effects. Not all phenomena are amenable to experimentation, for practical or ethical reasons (e.g. post-traumatic stress disorder, near-death experiences, effects of physical and social deprivation, etc.) Some phenomena (e.g. personality, age or sex differences) can only be investigated by methods which are, strictly speaking, quasi-experimental.

  17. Conclusion: Experiments are a useful tool for establishing cause and effect - but other methods (e.g. observation) are also important in science. A good experimental design ensures that the only variable that varies is the independent variable chosen by the experimenter - the effects of alternative confounding variables are eliminated (or at least rendered unsystematic by randomisation).

More Related