1 / 30

Evidence-based Practice in Psychology: Epistemological Diversity*

Evidence-based Practice in Psychology: Epistemological Diversity*. Steven D. Hollon Member, Presidential Task Force on EBPP Vanderbilt University Email: steven.d.hollon@vanderbilt.edu

kalin
Download Presentation

Evidence-based Practice in Psychology: Epistemological Diversity*

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Evidence-based Practice in Psychology: Epistemological Diversity* Steven D. Hollon Member, Presidential Task Force on EBPP Vanderbilt University Email: steven.d.hollon@vanderbilt.edu *Based upon: Report of the 2005 Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice: Policy statement: http://www.apa.org/practice/ebpstatement.pdf Complete report:http://www.apa.org/practice/ebpreport.pdf

  2. 2005 Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice (APA) Ronald F Levant EdD (Chair) Carol D Goodheart EdD (Chair) David H Barlow PhD Frederick L Newman PhD Jean Carter PhD John C Norcross PhD Karina Davidson PhD Doris K Silverman PhD Kristofer J Hagglund PhD Brian D Smedley PhD Steven D Hollon PhD Bruce E Wampold PhD Josephine D Johnson PhD Drew I Westen PhD Laura C Leviton PhD Brian T Yates PhD Alvin R Mahrer PhD Nolan W Zane PhD APA Staff: Geoffrey M Reed PhD Lynn F Bufka PhD Ernestine Penniman

  3. Basic Definition and Process • Based on IOM definition that emphasized integration of research evidence with clinical expertise and patient values • Drew on diverse group with range of expertise and interests from research scientists through clinical practitioners • Produced draft policy statement and position paper that was then posted for comments and subsequently revised • Approved by vote of council at the 2005 APA convention

  4. EBPP Defined • Evidence-based practice in psychology (EBPP) is the integration of the best available research with clinical expertise in the context of patient characteristics, culture, and preferences. • Closely parallels the definition adopted by the Institute of Medicine (2001) as adapted from Sackett and colleagues (2000, p. 14): “Evidence-based practice is the integration of best research evidence with clinical expertise and patient values.”

  5. Best Research Evidence • Evidence drawn from basic and applied research • Hierarchy from clinical observation through randomized controlled trials with respect to efficacy • Address efficacy and effectiveness (utility) • Absence of evidence not evidence of absence • Untested does not mean ineffective • Claims for efficacy should be tested *American Psychological Association (2002). Criteria for evaluating treatment guidelines. American Psychologist, 57, 1052-1059.

  6. Clinical Expertise • Encompasses number of competencies • positive therapeutic relationships • integration of diverse information • recognizes own bias and limitation • Derived from clinical and scientific training • Used to integrate research evidence with clinical data in context of patient preference

  7. Patient Characteristics, Values, and Context • Services most effective when responsive to patient problems, strengths, and preferences • Important variations in age, gender, race and ethnicity, and culture (among others) • EBPP seeks to maximize patient choice among effective alternative interventions

  8. Integration • Psychologist determines applicability of research evidence to particular patient • Application of research to given patient always involves probabilistic inferences • Continuous monitoring of patient progress and adjustment of treatment as needed • Clinical decisions made in collaboration with informed patient and in consideration of costs, benefits, and options available (never by untrained persons unfamiliar with specifics of the case)

  9. In Defense of RCTs • RCTs best way to detect causal influence • far from perfect but still the best we have • uncontrolled trials confound patients and procedures • hormone replacement therapy just latest example • need not do therapy like a scientist to evaluate effects • Carl Rogers one of the first to do controlled trials • good data best way to keep the critics at bay • no controlled trials before Eysenck’s critique • hundreds of subsequent trials show that psychotherapy works • some leading therapies still not adequately tested

  10. In Defense of ESTs • ESTs one reasonable way to see what works • look for well done studies that show effects • need not sacrifice external validity for internal validity • need not exclude representative patients (and no longer do) • can be used to test long-term treatments (and starting to do so) • do not mandate specificity but can detect it • treatment needs to work but not for reasons specified • special case for medications not for psychotherapy • treatment manuals neither necessary nor sufficient • need not constrain clinicians unduly if integrity maintained • merely useful aid for training and dissemination

  11. In Defense of ESTs • ESTs one reasonable way to see what works • look for well done studies that show effects • need not sacrifice external validity for internal validity • need not exclude representative patients (and no longer do) • can be used to test long-term treatments (and starting to do so) • do not mandate specificity but can detect it • treatment needs to work but not for reasons specified • special case for medications not for psychotherapy • treatment manuals neither necessary nor sufficient • need not constrain clinicians unduly if integrity maintained • merely useful aid for training and dissemination

  12. Must RCTs Exclude Representative Patients? • 805 patients evaluated • 240 (30%) randomized • 565 (70%) excluded • 235 (29%) low severity • 240 (30%) diagnostic • 96 (12%) psychosis • 63 (08%) sub abuse • 17 (02%) axis I • 19 (02%) axis II • 45 (06%) medical • 95 (10%) med refusal • 08 (01%) suicide risk From DeRubeis et al., 2005

  13. Must RCTs Exclude Complicated Patients? • 240 patients randomized • 40 (16%) depressed only • 200 (84%) comorbid • 146 (73%) axis I • 127 (53%) anxiety disorder • 86 (36%) sub abuse • 40 (16%) eating disorder • 125 (52%) axis II • 10 (04%) cluster a • 10 (04%) cluster b • 84 (35%) cluster c • 37 (16%) pd nos From DeRubeis et al., 2005

  14. In Defense of ESTs • ESTs one reasonable way to see what works • look for well done studies that show effects • need not sacrifice external validity for internal validity • need not exclude representative patients (and no longer do) • can be used to test long-term treatments (and starting to do so) • do not mandate specificity but can detect it • treatment needs to work but not for reasons specified • special case for medications not for psychotherapy • treatment manuals neither necessary nor sufficient • need not constrain clinicians unduly if integrity maintained • merely useful aid for training and dissemination

  15. CPT III Maintenance/Follow-up (36 months) Acute Treatment (1-18 months) Continuation (6-18 months) ADM and CT (N=225) ADM (N=90+) (monthly/ quarterly) (twice weekly/weekly) (monthly) No ADM (N=90+) 1st R a n d o m i z a t i o n 2nd R a n d o m i z a t i o n Response Relapse Recurrence ADM (N=225) ADM (N=90+) (monthly/ quarterly) (weekly/biweekly) (monthly) No ADM (N=90+) Remission Recovery

  16. 79% 64%

  17. 79% 69%

  18. 71% 41% 19% 09%

  19. Sustained Recovery = pRemit x cpRecover x 1-cpRecurrence

  20. In Defense of ESTs • ESTs one reasonable way to see what works • look for well done studies that show effects • need not sacrifice external validity for internal validity • need not exclude representative patients (and no longer do) • can be used to test long-term treatments (and starting to do so) • do not mandate specificity but can detect it • treatment needs to work but not for reasons specified • special case for medications not for psychotherapy • treatment manuals neither necessary nor sufficient • need not constrain clinicians unduly if integrity maintained • merely useful aid for training and dissemination

  21. Response to Treatment as a Function of Condition

  22. Continuation Followup

  23. Sustained Improvementfor All Assigned to Treatment

  24. Cumulative Direct Costs of ADM and CT

  25. In Defense of ESTs • ESTs one reasonable way to see what works • look for well done studies that show effects • need not sacrifice external validity for internal validity • need not exclude representative patients (and no longer do) • can be used to test long-term treatments (and starting to do so) • do not mandate specificity but can detect it • treatment needs to work but not for reasons specified • special case for medications not for psychotherapy • treatment manuals neither necessary nor sufficient • need not constrain clinicians unduly if integrity maintained • merely useful aid for training and dissemination

  26. Summary and Conclusions • Multiple components contribute to outcome • Patient, therapist, relationship also matter • Validate treatments to improve patient care • New methods emerge over time • Pursue other ways to improve care • Emphasize that which we can reliably teach

  27. Putting Things in Perspective • No one pretends that democracy is perfect or all-wise...indeed, it has been said that democracy is the worst form of Government except all those other forms that have been tried from time to time – Winston Churchill • The first principle for being a good psychologist is to not kid yourself, the second principle is to not kid anybody else – Paul Meehl

More Related