480 likes | 895 Views
Predicting secure infant attachment. Daniel Messinger, Ph.D. Review. In the presence of a consistent caregiver almost all infants form an attachment We’ve reviewed the classification of infant security of attachment in the strange situation.
E N D
Predicting secure infant attachment Daniel Messinger, Ph.D.
Review • In the presence of a consistent caregiver almost all infants form an attachment • We’ve reviewed the classification of infant security of attachment in the strange situation. • But what predicts a secure versus an insecure attachment? Messinger
Predicting attachment security • What different roles might infant temperament have in predicting security of attachment? What is the experimental evidence that caregiver sensitivity factors predicts secure attachment? What is the meta-analytic evidence that caregiver sensitivity factors predicts secure attachment? Messinger
Big picture • What produces secure attachment? • Infant – Temperament • Caregiver – Sensitivity • Social situation – divorce, daycare, social support • May affect infant directly • Situation - infant • Or affect infant indirectly: • Situation – caregiver sensitivity - infant Messinger
Infant Attachment and Maternal Depression • Mixed evidence • Some studies show effects, others do not • Chronicity of depression may be key • More consistent influence on day-to-day interaction • Sample study • Attachment insecurity significantly associated with maternal depression among infants and preschoolers. • Disorganized attachment especially common among mothers with more chronic depression. • Teti, Gelfand, Messinger, & Isabella (1995). Maternal depression and the quality of early attachment: An examination of infants, preschoolers, and their mothers. Developmental Psychology, 31(3), 364-376 Messinger
Orthodox View • Caregiver (Mother) Driven System • Sensitive caregiving yields secure attachment • Caregiver can adapt to any child temperament • Who’s has responsibility according to this systerm? Messinger
What is sensitivity? • Responsive • Understands and accepts the child’s individual proclivities • Orchestrates harmonious interactions • “especially involving the soothing of distress” • In a variety of situations • On a relatively consistent basis • Belsky, 1999, p. 249 Messinger
Just the right amount • Unresponsive caregiving Avoidant attachment • Attachment behaviors are suppressed (extinguished) • Sensitive caregiving Secure attachment • Attachment behaviors responded to appropriately • Inconsistent/intrusive caregiving Resistant attachment • Attachment behaviors only work when they are strong and insistent (intermittent reinforcement) • But little empirical evidence distinguishing parent behaviors distinguishing A & C Messinger
Mother or child? • Meta-analysis of 34 clinical studies indicates that maternal problems such as mental illness lead to more deviating attachment classification distributions than child problems such as deafness. • In clinical samples, the mother appears to play a more important role than the child in shaping the quality of the infant-mother attachment relationship • Van IJzendoorn, Goldberg, Kroonenberg, & Frenkel (1992). Messinger
Effects of child care on infant-mother attachment security • Significant effects of maternal sensitivity and responsiveness. • No significant effects of child-care experience (amount, age entry, or type of care) on attachment security or avoidance. • Interaction: more insecure when low maternal sensitivity/responsiveness combined with poor quality child care, more than minimal child care, or more than one care arrangement • 1,153 infants • NICHD study of early child care. Child Development. 1997. 68(5) 860-879 Messinger
Same at 36 months • No child-care factors (quantity, quality, or type) predicted attachment security • Maternal sensitivity was strongest predictor of preschool attachment classification. • Interaction: Low maternal sensitivity & more hours per week in care somewhat increased the risk of insecure (C). • Significant but modest stability of attachment classifications from 15 to 36 months • especially for children with A and C classifications. Messinger
Where does security lie? • In the infant or in the caregiver-infant dyad? • A meta-analysis of infant-father attachment shows weak but significant association between security of attachment to mother and father. • Does this suggest a role for temperament? Messinger
Temperament & Care giving • Child characteristics and care giving • Continuously and reciprocally impact each other in day-to-day interaction and development. • Little information on the process through which this occurs Seifer et al. • But lots of information on strength of respective caregiver and child influences Messinger
Two Temperamental Pathways • Indirect effect • Temperament Caregiver-Infant interaction Attachment security • Direct effect (Not empirically supported) • Temperament Strange Situation Behavior “Attachment Security” • Less prone to distress “Avoidant” • More prone to distress “Resistant” Messinger
Empirical resolution Avoidant Secure Resistant Temperament Calm---------------------------Irritable (A1 A2 or B1) B2 B3 (B4 or C1 C2) Caregiving Caregiving Belsky; Sussman-Stillman; several replications Messinger
Genetic and Caregiving-Based Contributions to Infant Attachment • Two contrasting explanations of differences in attachment: • Quality of infant-caregiver relationship • Reflection of infants’ temperament • Emphasis on emotional reactivity vs. emotion regulation • Proposed reconciliation: distress reactivity during SSP shaped by predispositions for negative emotionality Gangi
Genetic and Caregiving-Based Contributions to Infant Attachment • 155 infants and mothers • Measures of: • Maternal responsiveness at 6 months • Attachment at 12 and 18 months • Emotional distress in SSP • 5-HTTLPR variation • Raby et al., 2012 Gangi
Genetic and Caregiving-Based Contributions to Infant Attachment • Maternal responsiveness predicted attachment • 5-HTTLPR predicted distress during SSP • No prediction to attachment security, but subtypes • Genetic variation and caregiving context make unique contributions to differences in attachment behavior • Caregiving secure vs. insecure • 5-HTTLPR how this is manifested Gangi
Disorganized attachment predicted by parent behavior • Strongly related to parental maltreatment, & moderately related to sensitivity • Unrelated to difficult infant temperament • 2 studies have linked frightening parental behavior to disorganized attachment • Though not significantly related to depression • van IJzendoorn, M. H., Schuengel, C., & Bakermans Kranenburg, M. J. (1999). Disorganized attachment in early childhood: Meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Development and Psychopathology, 11(2), 225-249. Messinger
More evidence for care-giving effects • Experimental • Observational • Meta-analysis of quasi-experiments Messinger
Experiment 1: Sensitivity training • 100 irritable, low-SES Dutch infants • 50 mothers in experimental group • receive 3 home visits to foster “contingent, consistent, and appropriate responses to + and - infant signals” • 50 control mothers are observed only Messinger
Results • Experimental infants 36/50 (72%) secure • Control infants: 16/50 (32%) secure • Sensitivity training for mother decreases rates of insecurity among irritable infants • Meta-analysis of intervention studies showed a moderately large effect size, d = .48 • Van den Boom Messinger
Experiment 2: Replicate the Snuggly Effect! • 49 low-socioeconomic status (SES) mothers of newborn infants • Given soft baby carriers (more physical contact) or infant seats (less contact). • More experimental (83%) than control infants (38%) were securely attached at 13 mo. • 3.5 mo, mothers in the experimental group were more contingently responsive than control mothers to their infants' vocalizations. • Low cost experimentally-validated intervention? • Anisfeld, Casper, Nozyce, & Cunningham (1990). Does infant carrying promote attachment? An experimental study of the effects of increased physical contact on the development of attachment. Child Development, 61(5), 1617-1627. Messinger
Conclusions • Sensitivity is important • Temperament may also be a factor • Does sensitive interaction make a difference in naturalistic settings • Many studies have been done • using many measures of interaction • Meta-analysis can help sort them out Messinger
Overall (No Grouping) • All caregiving comparisons • 7,223 infants in 123 comparisons • 17% greater likelihood of security • r = .17 • Random sample with no overlapping comparisons • 4,176 infants in 66 comparisons/studies • 19% greater likelihood of security (r = .19) Messinger
Sensitivity Studies Only • Perceive signals accurately and respond promptly and appropriately • 22% (r = .22), 7,223 infants in 123 comparisons • Original Ainsworth subscale • 24% (r = .24), 837 infants in subset of 16 studies • Socioeconomic class is a moderator • Middle (r = .27); lower (r = .15) Messinger
Conclusions • Sensitivity and quality of interaction are important and consistent (but not exclusive) predictors of attachment security. • Sensitivity important but not only factor • Orthodox hypothesis supported weakly Messinger
However, interactions with genes reported by Barry et al. 2008 Messinger
Autism challenges attachment theory • 55 toddlers with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), mental retardation, language delay, and typical development. • diagnosed at 4 years. Two years before, attachment, sensitivity assessed • Parents of children with ASD =y sensitive as other parents • But children show more disorganization, less involvement. • More sensitive parents had more secure children, • but only in group without ASD. • Less severe autistic symptoms in the social domain predicted more attachment security.. • van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Rutgers, A. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van Daalen, E., Dietz, C., Buitelaar, J. K., et al. (2007). Parental sensitivity and attachment in children with autism spectrum disorder: Comparison with children with mental retardation, with language delays, and with typical development. Child Development, 78, 597-608. Messinger
Subtle attachment differences:More distance security 27% 42% 73% 58% Haltigan et al., 2010
FFSF Mid-range models based on Beebe et al. • Predictive modeling will examine the competing ability of linear and nonlinear midrange models of infant-mother vocal coordination to predict attachment security (see Beebe et al., 2010 & Jaffe et al., 2001). In pilot analyses based on continuous ratings of affect, for example, mid-range levels of mother responsiveness (infant-to-mother interactive influence) were associated with 15-month attachment security, the highest levels were associated with resistance, and the lowest levels with avoidance and disorganized attachment classifications, F(3,23) = 3.55, p = .03, estimated ω2 = .24. Messinger
Sensitivity • Sensitive caregiving promotes attachment. • But effects are not universally found • Seifer et al., 1996 • Sensitive caregiving is underspecified • What does sensitivity look like cross-culturally and in different subcultures? Messinger
Attachment & social play • What does sensitivity look like in different caregiving domains such as playmate and attachment figure? • Attachment theory is not clear as to whether the concepts are distinguishable and what type of association is to be expected. Messinger
Variability within the family • Caregivers occupy many roles vis-à-vis the child: playmate, discipliner, as well as attachment figures • What does sensitive caregiving look like in different domains as parents occupy these different roles? • Meta-analysis of link between sensitive fathering and attachment showed weak but significant association (d = .13). Messinger
Cross-cultural evidence • Among dyads living in subsistence societies secure attachment exists in relationships in which social play between caregiver and child was not observed and was seen as frivolous. • Gusii, Ganda, etc. • Secure attachment without play • The anthropological veto Messinger
Middle-class American dyads • Marginal prediction from early quality of social play to later security of attachment (Ainsworth et al., Kiser) • Moderate associations between concurrent social play and attachment • Roggman’s secure dyads showed more: infant-initiated toy exchanges & maternal positive vocalizations • co-orientation of attention to toys (males only) • Rosenberg’s secure dyads spend more time reciprocally interacting Messinger
Low SES American mother-infant dyads • Egeland shows weak antecedent association • Multivariate but only 3 of 12 univariate • E.g., only satisfaction in play • Gaensbauer shows no significant association in • infant social use of objects • mother response to infant bids • infant positive affect • (n = 107) • MLS Study Messinger
Meta-analytic results • De Wolff and van Ijzendoorn also found that Ainsworth-based measures of sensitivity were stronger predictors of attachment security in middle-class r = .27) than lower-class dyads (r = .15). Messinger
Variability • Characterizes the association between security of attachment and quality of social play cross-culturally Messinger
‘Limited relations between attachment security and quality of social interaction.’ Messinger
Attachment as organizer • Attachment is pre-eminent "affective bond" that organizes interaction between infant and caregiver (organizational construct perspective) • If attachment is secure, • Positive play should be possible or • play should be positive Messinger
The Paradox of Sensitivity • Strongly predictive of many outcomes • But somewhat subjective in content • A joystick resolution? Messinger
In a teaching situation, student non-experts rate teaching even if you ask them to rate supportiveness • Sensitive structuring (“the degree to which the parent is involved in providing appropriate structure and teaching for the child”), • ICC = .75, and concordance with expert ratings, r = .71 • But emotional supportiveness (“the degree to which the parent is warm, positive, responsive and supportive to her child, while also respecting the child’s independence”). • ICC = .47, r = .36, ns. • In fact, non-expert emotional supportiveness ratings exhibited high associations with the structuring ratings of experts, • r =.78, p < .001, and non-experts, r =.70, p < .01. Messinger
Sensitivity in the SS • Lower levels of maternal sensitivity (Behrens, Parker, & Haltigan, 2011; Leerkes, Parade, & Gudmundson, 2011; Smith & Pederson, 1988) and more frequent maternal displays of atypical behaviors (e.g., lower responsivity, inappropriate responses to child affect, disrupted communication; Goldberg, Benoit, Blokland, & Madigan, 2003; Goldberg, MacKay-Soroka, & Rochester, 1994; Lyons-Ruth, Bronfman, & Parsons, 1999) have been documented most often amongst insecurely attached children and children with disorganized attachment when maternal behavior and child attachment were assessed concurrently in the SSP Messinger
References • Interactional and contextual determinants of attachment security (Belsky, 1999) • The Nature of the Child’s Ties (Cassidy, 1999) • Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment (De Wolff & van IJzendoorn, 1997) • Van IJzendoorn, Goldberg, Kroonenberg, & Frenkel (1992). The relative effects of maternal and child problems on the quality of attachment - a meta-analysis of attachment in clinical-samples. Child Development, 63, 840-858. • Van IJzendoorn, & Kroonenberg (1988). Cross-cultural patterns of attachment: A meta-analysis of the strange situation. Child Development, 59, 147-156.1) van den Boom DC. Do first-year intervention effects endure? Follow-up during toddlerhood of a sample of Dutch irritable infants. Child Development 1995;66(6):1798-1816. • 2) van den Boom DC. The influence of temperament and mothering on attachment and exploration: an experimental manipulation of sensitive responsiveness among lower-class mothers with irritable infants Child Development 1994;65(5):1457-77. 65(6): Messinger