280 likes | 302 Views
The Phonaestheme /gl/:. To what extent are English speakers aware of a meaning connection with ‘ light ’ ?. Michael Willett Cardiff University, UK WillettMD@cardiff.ac.uk. Research supported by. The Context of Phonaesthemes. Begins with J. R. Firth ’ s idea of prosodies
E N D
The Phonaestheme /gl/: To what extent are English speakers aware of a meaning connection with ‘light’? Michael WillettCardiff University, UKWillettMD@cardiff.ac.uk Research supported by
The Context of Phonaesthemes • Begins with J. R. Firth’s idea of prosodies • Prosodies are any functional units of sound that are not abstracted to the problematic level of discrete, individual sounds – i.e. the phoneme. Prosodies include intonation system, the syllable, etc. • Firth more convinced by idea of words as [groups of] syllables, themselves products of of phonetically-allowable ‘beginnings’(onsets), middles (nuclei) and endings (codas) in the language than as a sumof individual, discrete phonemes
Word analysed as series of Prosodies Gleam /m/ = coda prosody /gl/ = onset prosody /i:/ = vowel nucleus
Defining Phonaesthemes • Firth (1930; 1957):A phonaestheme is what is formed when a prosody takes on ‘a cumulative suggestive value’– a connotation – for speakers, as a result of recurring in a range of English words that denote a broadly similar meaning • That is, speakers ‘hear and learn to make these sounds… [in similar] contexts of experience’ (1930:184-185).
Onset /gl/ • Most widely-cited English phonaestheme • In English, the most common meaning denoted by words beginning /gl/ is ‘light’, or semantically-related ideas, including ‘sight’(Sturtevant, 1947; Marchand, 1966; Bowles, 1995; Sadowski, 2001)
How frequently recurring? • 19 of the 59 (32%) head lexemes beginning /gl/ listed in the Compact OED denote a meaning related to ‘light’: • Glade, glamour, glance, glare, glass, glaucoma, glaze, gleam, glimmer, glimpse, glint, glisten, glister, glitter, glitz, gloaming, gloom, gloss, glow
Defining Phonaesthemes • Phonaesthemes are not:Onomatopoeic (they do not mime in sound their denotations) - /gl/ cannot mime ‘light’!Iconic(there appears no conceived similarity between/gl/ and the real-world idea of ‘light’ that wouldmake it more suitable than any other prosody)Universal(every language will have its ownphonaesthemes, because theyarise from forms’ denotations)
Phonaesthemes and Arbitrariness • The existence of phonaesthemes does not pose a challenge to Saussure’s idea of the arbitrariness of linguistic signs • There is nothing initially about /gl/ to have made it any more suitable for denoting ‘light’ than any other prosody – e.g. /sp/ or /tr/ or /kw/ etc. • Phonaesthemes are diachronically non-arbitrary (over time they become non arbitrary and “more suitable” for use in/gl/ words by analogy to the existing,arbitrary forms)
Existing Studies into /gl/ phonaestheme • All existing studies into /gl/ phonaestheme are “theoretical”; they look at the existing vocabulary of a language (usually English), and calculate how many of the /gl/-onset words denote a meaning of ‘light’ • However, this is not sufficient grounds to conclude that a /gl/ phonaestheme actually exists in that language…
Existing Studies into /gl/ phonaestheme • …because phonaesthemes, as defined by Firth, involve speakers ascribing connotations to prosodies as a result of the contexts in which those prosodies frequently recur • Thus, in the words of Jespersen (1922a: 408), ‘the suggestiveness of [these sounds] as felt by present-day speakers…must be taken into account’ • No study to date addresses the /gl/-phonaestheme from speakers’perspectives
Research Question • Do native English speakers connote the meaning ‘light’ more frequently than any other meaning, from words beginning with /gl/?
Overview of Methodology • Three experiments given to 30 native English speakers, in the form of an online survey • Experimental cues taken from Abelin’s (1999) investigation into Swedish phonaesthemes – the only researchto date that studies phonaesthemesproductively • Two experiments are closed-ended multiple choice questions; one is fullyopen-ended.
Experiment 1 • Respondents provided with audio recordings of three coined (‘nonsense’) words, and one image • The image depicts glittering, reflected sparkles of light • Only one of the ‘nonsense’ words features onset /gl/. The other two feature different onsets. All three feature identical nuclei and codas, tocontrol for any extraneous phonaesthetic effects these prosodies may have.
Experiment 1 (cont’d) • Respondents asked to listen to the audio recordings of ‘nonsense’ words, and choose whichever word they feel is most appropriate to name a specified part of the content in the image (the ‘light’ part) • If /gl/ phonaestheme is recognised by speakers, respondents should show a degree of preferencefor the /gl/-onset word to name the content inthe image;the onsets are the only dimension on which each word varies from the others
gless /ɡlɛs/ pless /plɛs/ fless /flɛs/
Experiment 2 • Designed to provide a re-test and add reinforcing evidence to that collected in Experiment 1 • Reverses the process of Experiment 1: Now, respondents provided with three images and single audio recording of a coined (‘nonsense’) word • Coined word features the /gl/ onset plus randomly-chosen nucleus and coda • Only one of the images depicts a meaning associated with ‘light’. The other images depict unrelated meanings
In theory, respondents should attribute coined word to this image (B), as it depicts glowing, shining stars gliss /glıs/
Experiment 3 • Fully open-ended • Respondents provided with a definition relating to the emission of light, and asked to coin (‘invent’) any word of their choosing to name this definition • If /gl/ phonaestheme recognised by speakers,respondents should prefer coining words with/gl/- to any other onset
Experiment 1: Results • 25/30 respondents (83%) preferred using the coined word featuring /gl/- to describe the glittering eye-shadow
Experiment 2: Results • 27/30 respondents (90%) preferred attributing the glittering stars image to the /gl/- word
Experiment 3: Results • 12/30 (40%) respondents coined a word beginning /gl/ in response to definition • Given that there are at least 81 possible word onsets in English (Kreidler, 2004; Cruttenden, 2008); the fact that 40% of respondents all selected the same consonant cluster seems particularly high in a 30-strong respondent cohort…
Experiment 3: Results (cont’d) • Chart showing the frequency with which onsets were used by speakers in response to open-ended question:
Is the phonaesthetic effect of /gl/ affected by the vowel(s) with which it is paired? • Experiment 2 sees a slight increase over Experiment 1 in the number of speakers associating /gl/ to the meaning of ‘light’ (compare 83% to 90%). • Experiments 1 and 2 use coined /gl/- words which are phonetically identical aside from their vowel nuclei:/glɛs/ in Experiment 1 (mid-front vowel)/glıs/ in Experiment 2 (high-front vowel)
Is the phonaesthetic effect of /gl/ affected by the vowel(s) with which it is paired? • Further evidence that respondents prefer /gl/+high-front vowel from the words coined in Experiment 3 (insofar as the vowel graphemes can be interpreted)…
Is the phonaesthetic effect of /gl/ affected by the vowel(s) with which it is paired? • Examining the /gl/ words coined by speakers in Experiment 3, and their inferred pronunciation:gloosie gliss /ɡlıs/gleety /ɡliːtiː/glite glinkel /ɡlınkəl/glissipel /ɡlısıpɛl/ or /ɡliːsıpɛl/ if second <s> in second syllableglizon glizzen /ɡlızən/ or /ɡliːzən/ (as above)glind glisty /ɡlıstiː/glissiant /ɡlısıənt/ or /ɡliːsıənt/ (as above) glastifer *at least 7 of the 12 words coined feature a high, front vowel – either /ı/ or /iː/
Concluding Remarks • /gl/ is consistently and strongly associated with ‘light’ by speakers across all experiments, showing evidence that English native speakers recognise its phonaesthetic function • However, it appears it has an even stronger association with ‘light’ for speakers if combined with a high-front vowel Experiment 2, with high front vowel nucleus, yields 7% more responses associating the phonaestheme to a meaning of ‘light’ than experiment 1, with mid-front vowel At least 7 of the 12 respondents who coin a /gl/ word in Experiment 3 use a high-front vowel • This finding seems to support Jespersen’s (1922b)claim that in many languages, high-front vowelstend to be associated with ‘lightness’and low back vowels ‘darkness’
References • Abelin, A (1999) Studies in Sound Symbolism. PhD thesis: Göteborg University. • Bowles, H (1995) “The Semantic Properties of the Phonaestheme”. Studi italiani di linguistica teorica ed applicata, 1: 91-106. • Cruttenden, A (2008) Gimson’s Pronunciation of English. London: Hodder Education. • Firth, J R (1930) Speech. In Firth, J R, The Tongues of Men and Speech. London: Oxford University Press. • Firth, J R (1957) Papers in Linguistics 1934-1951. London: Oxford University Press. • Hayes, B (2009) Introductory Phonology. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. • Jespersen, O (1922a) Language: Its Nature, Development and Origin. London: George Allen & Unwin Ltd. • Jespersen, O (1922b) “Symbolic Value of the Vowel I”. In Jespersen, O, Selected Writings of Otto Jespersen. Abingdon: Routledge. • Kreidler, C W (2004) The Pronunciation of English. Oxford: Blackwell. • Marchand, H (1966) The Categories and Types of Present-Day English Word- Formation. Alabama: University of Alabama Press. • Sadowski, P (2001) “The sound as an echo to the sense: The iconicity of English gl- words”. In Fischer, O and Nanny, M (eds.) The Motivated Sign. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. • Sturtevant, E H (1947) An Introduction to Linguistic Science. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.