580 likes | 596 Views
Cognitive Disabilities. General Cognitive Disability. Behaviourally diagnosed IQ Average = 100, standard deviation = 15 Only 5% of population has cognitive disabilities Levels Mild (IQ 50-70); about 85% of cases Moderate (IQ 35-50) Severe (IQ 20-35) Profound (IQ below 20).
E N D
General Cognitive Disability • Behaviourally diagnosed • IQ • Average = 100, standard deviation = 15 • Only 5% of population has cognitive disabilities • Levels • Mild (IQ 50-70); about 85% of cases • Moderate (IQ 35-50) • Severe (IQ 20-35) • Profound (IQ below 20)
General Cognitive Disabilities • Genetics relatively well studied • Especially for serious (IQ<50) cognitive disabilities • Can now investigate specific disabilities at gene level • Mild disabilities (candidates for QTLs) are harder
Quantitative Genetics • Sibs of mildly cognitive disabled have below average IQ scores • Sibs of severely cognitively disabled do not • Why?
Mild Disabilities: Genetic or Environmental? • Genetics seems to be important • Concordances for mild cognitive disabilities: MZ 75%, DZ 46% • High/low IQ equally heritable across age (infancy to middle age) • But few scores in these studies go below IQ 70 • ~50% mildly disabled children also have behavioural problems • Quantitative range of normal distribution?
Single Gene Disorders • Over 250 known disorders that show low IQ (also other, often primary, symptoms) • Generally quite rare
PKU • Single gene autosomal recessive • Defect in PAH gene (chr. 12) for enzyme phenylalanine hydroxylase • Converts amino acid phenylalanine to other essential compounds • Over 400 disease causing mutations of PAH • About 1 in 10,000-15,000 births • Screening of infants at birth since 1961 • Environmental fix; restrict access to proteins (and some other foods) and supplement amino acids lacking from no protein diet
Neurofibromatosis Type 1 • Member of the neurocutaneous syndromes, all producing neurologic and dermatologic lesions • Autosomal dominant disorder from mutation of gene NF1 (chr. 17) • NF1 encodes neurofibromin protein, involved in intracellular signaling • Cognitive and learning disabilities also common
X-Linked • Genes on X chromosome • Tend to effect males • Females: X-inactivation --> mosaicism • Females with disorder, often show highly variable severity depending on how many/which cells affected
X-inactivation • One X copy “silenced” in early blastocyst • Packaged into transcriptionally inactive heterochromatin • Prevents twice as many X chromosome gene products being produced as in the male • In placental mammals, which X chromosome inactivated is random
Default state is inactivation for X chr. • In XXX individuals, still only one active X • Autosomally-encoded “blocking factor” hypothesized; binds to one X and prevents its silencing • X-inactivation centre (XIC) necessary and sufficient for silencing • Translocating XIC to autosomal chromosome causes inactivation of that autosome; X lacking XIC not inactivated
Fragile X Syndrome • About 1 in 3600 males, 1 in 4000-6000 females • Mutation of the FMR1 gene on the X chr. • Normal FMR1: 6 to 55 repeats of CGG codon. • Fragile X: 230+ repeats • Expansion of CGG --> methylation of that portion of DNA --> silences expression of FMR1 protein • Methylation constricts the chromosome; “fragile” appearance under the microscope
Fragile X • Males have one copy of X chromosome; those with expanded FMR1 are symptomatic • Cognitive disabilities and physical features (elongated face, large ears, low muscle tone) • Females’ two Xs doubles chances of having functioning FMR1 allele • With X-inactivation females with 1 expanded FMR1 allele can have some symptoms
Rett Syndrome • Deceleration of growth of head, hands, and feet, cognitive impairment, GI disorders, serious verbal deficits • Sporadic (de novo) mutations to gene MECP2 on X chromosome • Almost always affects females; male fetuses rarely survive to term or die at very early age • With one functioning gene, female fetuses can produce enough proteins to survive to birth, but symptoms manifest between 6-18 months post-birth
Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy • Mutation of gene on X chromosome that codes for dystrophin protein • Necessary structural component of muscle tissue • Progressive muscle weakness and loss of muscle mass; some cognitive disfunction • Affects males • Females can be carriers; rarely manifest symptoms • Fatal by age 30
Lesch-Nyhan Syndrome • X-linked recessive; rare, 1 in 380,000 live births • Deficiency of enzyme hypoxanthine-guanine phosphoribosyltransferase (HGPRT) • Causes build up of uric acid in all body fluids • Severe mental and physical deficits; self-mutilating behaviour common • Female carriers typically asymptomatic; may have some difficulties from increased uric acid excretion
Chromosomal Abnormalities • More common • Generally moderate to severe disability • 7% of children with unexplained moderate or severe disabilities have detectable deletions vs. 0.5% of children with mild disabilities • Additional chromosomes • More frequent; easily detectable… • Deletions of parts of chromosomes • Use of microarrays identifying more of these • Might ultimately be more common
Williams Syndrome • Small deletion (chr. 7) • Affects about 20 genes; elastin production, LIM kinase, and others • Usually spontaneous mutations • Connective tissue defects, multiple medical problems, generally moderate cognitive disfunction; some similarities to autism
Angelman Syndrome • Genetic imprinting; inherited from mother • Deletion of genes at 15q11-13 • E.g., defect/deletion in gene UBE3A, coding for an ubiquitin ligase; absence results in defects in hippocampus and cerebellum • Usually novel in formation of gametes • Risk to sibs of proband is ~1% • Moderate cognitive disfunction, motor and speech impairments, seizures, inappropriate happy demenor
Prader-Willi Syndrome • Genetic imprinting; from father • Deletion or defect of up to 7 genes at 15q11-13 • Effect is on some non-coding RNA; modifies other non-coding RNAs and mRNA processing • Low IQ, compulsive overeating
Down Syndrome • Most common cause of cognitive disability (about 1/1000 births) • Trisomy 21 • Over 300 symptoms; physical, behavioural, and cognitive • Mean IQ ~55; dementia commonly sets in by age 45
XXY male 1/750; low testosterone in adolescence Small testes, enlarged breast tissue, infertility, low IQ, poor speech and language XXX 1/1000 females Mean IQ ~85, poor on verbal tasks XYY 1/1000 males Speech deficits, language and reading difficulties; tendency to be tall X0 1/2500 females 99% miscarry; accounts for 10% of spontaneous abortions Short stature, abnormal sexual development, verbal IQ normal but performance IQ ~90 after adolescence Sex Chromosome Abnormalities
XYY Males • 1961: first described case, karyotype of man whose son had Down Syndrome • 1965: 7 of 197 inmates in a Scottish maximum security prison are XYY; unusually tall and aggressive. “Super-male Syndrome” • 1968: Murderer of Parisian prostitutes found to be XYY • 1968: Richard Speck (murdered 8 nursing students in Chicago) reported to be XYY; subsequently rejected • 1968: 23 studies published examining rates of XYY in prisons, 0 studying XYY in the general population • 1969: study screens inmates for XYY (but only karyotype tall, violent ones); finds 1 of the 9 tested is XYY • 1969: first good study on base rates of chromosomal abnormalities conduced (about 1/1000) • 1970: Newsweek article on XYY “Congenital Criminals”, Y becomes the “Crime Chromosome”
Dale & Harley (1980) Mental Institute Penal Institute Mental-Penal Men karyotyped 5100 6983 5479 XYY 13 31 104 XYY/1000 2.5 4.4 20.0
Nanko et al. (1979) Sample Size <100 101-200 201-500 >500 Number of Studies 6 9 3 5 Karyotypes 326 1287 1199 2838 XYYs found 4 10 6 7 XYY/1000 12.3 7.8 5.0 2.4
Guesstimate • If 1/1000 males is XYY and 5/1000 inmates is XYY, then what proportion of XYY males are in jail? • Rough guess = 0.5% • Based on knowing proportion of population which is incarcerated in Western countries
Why? • Some hypotheses: • 1. They are violent • 2. They are tall • 3. They have cognitive disabilities (lower intelligence)
Violent • No personality or crime differences between XYY and XY inmates (Clark et al., 1970) • XYY actually seem to be less violent than XY inmates • More likely incarcerated for property than violent crime • Also, XXY are disproportionately incarcerated, and their crime pattern is like that of XYY (Hook, 1979)
Tall • Tall XY are not jailed disproportionately to other XY (Hook & Kim, 1971)
Lower Intelligence • 31,436 men born in Copenhagen in 1944-47 • IQ tested during mandatory military service • Studied all tall (>184 cm) men • Karyotyped 4,139 of them • Used criminal history database and school exam records
Findings • Karyotype finds 12 XYY and 16 XXY • Base crime conviction rate of 9.3% • 5 XYY had records (non-violent offences), 3 XXY had records • Intelligence test scores were low for XYY and XXY; convicted XYY and XXY even lower • XY males with records also had lower IQ test scores
Inference • Cognitive deficits are important • XYY not necessarily any more violent or dangerous • Cognitive deficits limit employment options • May be “poor” criminals - more likely to get caught, miss plea bargain opportunities, etc., possibly due to lower IQ
Quantitative Genetics • Mild cognitive disability likely quantitative, not qualitative • Polygenic • QTL hypothesis • Many genes making small contributions • Many disorders, then, may simply be the “low end” of the normal distribution of the population’s traits
DF Extremes Analysis • Method for estimating heritability of disorders that are defined qualitatively (i.e., either have or don’t have the disorder) • But disorders depend on an underlying trait that varies quantitatively along a continuum
DF Extremes Analysis • Developed by DeFries and Fulker • A regression-based method for analyzing twin data • Designed for proband-selected data • At least one twin has an extreme score • Based on regression to the mean
Regression to the Mean • 1000 individuals tested on some measure
Regression to the Mean • Test same 1000 individuals one month later; gives a similar profile
Regression to the Mean • Focus on people who scored very low at time 1 (extreme group) • Can we predict how this group should score at time 2?
Regression to the Mean • Mean at time 2 for extreme group from time 1 depends on the correlation between the two time points • Imagine correlations of +1, 0, and something in intermediate
Regression to the Mean Time 1 Correlation = +1 Time 2 Time 1 Correlation = 0 Time 2 Time 1 Intermediate correlation Time 2 Pop. mean
DF Extremes Analysis • Considers the differential regression towards the mean between MZ and DZ twins • To parallel the example, time 1 is the proband score and time 2 is the co-twin score
Implementation • E.g., select individuals in low 5% of a trait • Look at the scores of their co-twins • If a trait is influenced by genes, MZ co-twins should not regress towards the mean as much as DZ co-twins • Due to higher correlation between MZ twins
Visualization Probands MZ co-twins DZ co-twins MZ co-twin mean DZ co-twin mean Proband mean Pop. mean
Heritability and Non-shared Environment • Regression to the mean reflects info about the correlation between twins. • Standardize means so population mean becomes 0 and the proband mean is 1 • Then transformed MZ and DZ co-twin means can be implemented just as MZ and DZ correlations are. • 2 x diff. b/t MZ & DZ co-twin mean estimates heritability and 1 minus the MZ co-twin mean estimates non-shared environment
Graphically Proband mean MZ co-twin mean DZ co-twin mean Population mean e2 h2/2 h2/2 + c2 1 0 e = unique environment c = shared environment
Learning Disorders • Dyslexia • ~10% of children have difficulty learning to read • 80% of diagnosed learning disorders have difficulty reading • Also, math disorder (moderate heritability from twin studies) co-occurs with reading disorder • Sibs and parents of reading-disabled probands do worse on tests of reading ability • Study of 250 twins with one reading disabled: 66% concordance for MZ, 36% for DZ
Verbal Ability is Heritable 1.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 Unique envir. Shared envir. Heredity Early childhood Middle childhood Adolescence Adulthood Old Age Meta-Analysis of recent twin studies (Adapted from: Price (2002))
Specific Language Impairment (SLI) 1.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 MZ twins Risk DZ twins 1st degree relatives Controls Relations Data from Stromswold (1998) and Stromswold (2001) (Figure adapted from Price (2002))
Vernes et al. (2008) • Rare mutations of FOXP2 transcription factor cause some forms of SLI • Genome screened for regions bound by FOXP2 • Tested for SNPs in set of 184 families with SLI • FOXP2 binds to and dramatically down-regulates CNTNAP2 gene • One of largest genes in human genome (~1.5% of chr. 7) • Role in cell adhesion molecules in developing human cortex and axon differentiation • Significant quantitative association with nonsense-word repetition
Reading Disability • Causes of individual variation throughout the population may be different from the causes of differences between a group with extreme scores and the rest of the population • Genetic contribution to variation in reading ability does not mean illiterates are genetically different from the rest of the population • MZ twins correlate 0.9, DZ twins correlate 0.65 • h2 ~ 0.5 (group heritability)