1 / 50

Explaining C o l o u r Term Typology with an Evolutionary Model

Explaining C o l o u r Term Typology with an Evolutionary Model . Mike Dowman 24 November, 2005. Colour and Colour Words. Colour is a continuous three dimensional space . Colour words denote regions of colour Is colour naming completely arbitrary?.

pepin
Download Presentation

Explaining C o l o u r Term Typology with an Evolutionary Model

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Explaining Colour Term Typology with an Evolutionary Model Mike Dowman 24 November, 2005

  2. Colour and Colour Words • Colour is a continuous three dimensional space • Colour words denote regions of colour • Is colour naming completely arbitrary?

  3. Colour Term Typology There are clear typological patterns in how languages name colour. • neurophysiology of the vision system • colours in the environment • or cultural factors? • Constraints on learnable languages • or an evolutionary process?

  4. Basic Colour Terms Most studies look at a subset of all colour terms: • Terms must be psychologically salient • Known by all speakers • Meanings are not predictable from the meanings of their parts • Don’t name a subset of colours named by another term

  5. Number of Basic Terms English has red, orange, yellow, green, blue, purple, pink, brown, grey, black and white. crimson, blonde, taupe are not basic. All languages have 2 to 11 basic terms • Except Russian and Hungarian • Pirahã arguably has none • The concept of basic colour term is disputed

  6. Prototypes Colour terms have good and marginal examples  prototype categories • People disagree about the boundaries of colour word denotations • But agree on the best examples – the prototypes Berlin and Kay (1969) found that this was true both within and across languages.

  7. The Munsell Array

  8. English and Berinmo Colour Mappings

  9. purple pink orange grey green yellow white black red blue brown Berlin and Kay’s Implicational Hierarchy. Berlin and Kay (1969) Small set of possible colour term systems 98 Languages in study Only Cantonese, Vietnamese, Western Apache, Hopi, Samal and Papago didn’t fit the hierachy

  10. World Colour Survey 110 minor languages (Kay, Berlin, Merrifield, 1991; Kay et al 1997; Kay and Maffi, 1999) • All surveyed using Munsell arrays Black, white, red, yellow, green and blue seem to be fundamental colours • They are more predictable than derived terms (orange, purple, pink, brown and grey)

  11. white-red-yellow + black-green-blue white + red-yellow + black-green-blue white + red-yellow + black + green-blue white + red + yellow + black + green-blue white + red + yellow + black + green + blue white + red + yellow + black-green-blue white + red + yellow + green + black-blue white + red + yellow-green-blue + black white + red + yellow-green + blue + black Evolutionary Trajectories

  12. Derived Terms • Brown and purple terms often occur together with green-blue composites • Orange and pink terms don’t usually occur unless green and blue are separate • But sometimes orange occurs without purple • Grey is unpredictable • No attested turquoise or lime basic terms

  13. Exceptions and Problems • 83% of languages on main line of trajectory • 25 languages were in transition between stages • 6 languages didn’t fit trajectories at all  Kuku-Yalanji (Australia) has no consistent term for green • Waorani (Ecuador) has a yellow-white term that does not include red • Gunu (Cameroon) contains a black-green-blue composite and a separate blue term

  14. Criticism of Kay • Much more variability than Kay suggests – both within and across languages • Criteria for distinguishing basic colour terms don’t work • Colour is often conflated with other properties: texture, variegation, etc. • Colour words can only be understood in relation to the rest of the language • Colour words have religious and cultural significance  Saunders (1992), MacLaury (1997), Levinson (2001), MacKeigan (2005)

  15. Universal Foci Frequency Distribution of 10,644 WCS Colour-term Foci (MacLaury, 1997)

  16. Why are there Universal Foci? Unique hues • red and green • yellow and blue Opposite colours: • Afterimages • Mixing opposites colours produces grey

  17. Rosch-Heider’s Studies Foci are more salient • Children tend to pick focal colours when given a free choice • Dugum Dani speakers found it easier to learn to associate words with foci Most other results of Rosch can be attributed to properties of the Munsell array used in her experiments

  18. Neurophysiological Basis for Unique Hues Opponent cells  Each cell responds maximally to red, yellow, green or blue • And minimally to the opposite colour Kay and McDaniel (1978) proposed that the maximal firing rates of these cells explained the universal foci

  19. Problems with Neurophysiological Explanation • Null point of red-green opponents is at greenish-yellow • Cells respond to achromatic light • Cells responding to red don’t respond to low wavelength violet colours • Inter-subject variation in sensitivity to opponent channels does not correlate with inter-subject variation in which hues are unique

  20. Predicting Colour Terms from Foci Hypothesis: • green and blue are close together  green-blue composites are common • turquoise never occurs • red and blue are far apart • red-blue composites never occur • purple is common Are they the correct distances between foci?

  21. Conceptual Colour Spaces • Similarity of colours is not predictable from physical properties of light • Several perceptually standard colour spaces (Munsell, CIE L*a*b*, Optical Society of America, etc.) • In Munsell red and blue are the most dissimilar • In CIE L*a*b* they are the most similar • The relative distances between universal foci vary greatly between colour spaces • Is there really a correct psychological colour solid?

  22. Colour Space in the Model red - 7 yellow - 19 blue - 30 green - 26 Colour space is 40 units in size

  23. red red red yellow yellow yellow blue blue blue green green green U1 U2 Bayesian inference of colour term system naming of colours Iterated Learning Models Models simulate the transmission of language between agents (artificial people) • Each agent can learn a language based on utterances spoken by another agent • In turn they can speak and so create data from which another agent can learn

  24. Tony Belpaeme (2002) • Ten agents • Colour categories represented with adaptive networks • CIE-L*a*b* colour space used • Multi or single-generational • Communication or individual development of colour words

  25. Guessing game Speaker tries to find a word that names a topic colour but not context colours • If this fails it modifies its colour categories to increase their discriminative potential • Otherwise word and topic + context shown to the hearer • If hearer can distinguish topic from context, word-category association strengthened. • Otherwise hearer is shown the correct topic, and adapts its colour category

  26. Emergent Languages • Coherent colour categories emerged that were shared by all the agents • Colour space divided into a number of regions – each named by a different colour word • But some variation between speakers • And no explanation of Typology

  27. Belpaeme and Bleys (in press) Colour terms represented using points in the colour space Colours chosen from natural scenes, or at random

  28. Results Locations of centres of emergent colour categories correlated with those seen in the World Colour Survey  Most clustered in a few parts of the colour space  Similarity was greatest when communication was simulated • And when example colours selected randomly Shape of colour space but not colours in the environment helps to explain typology

  29. My Colour Model • Learns using Bayesian inference • Agents simply name colours (no feedback given about success of learning) • Simplified 1 dimensional colour space • Ten agents • Multiple generations

  30. Learning Colour Terms • Each colour term is learned independently to any others • Data is a set of example colours • Universal foci are especially salient  Agents are more likely to remember examples of universal foci than of other colours

  31. Bayesian Learning low probability hypothesis high probability hypothesis medium probability hypothesis

  32. Urdu

  33. Nol: 15, 18, 23 Wor: 38, 5, 11 Mehi: 25, 28, 30, 35 Agent Communication Agent 3 Agent 8 Agent 3 thinks Mehi is the best label for colour 27 Mehi: 27 remembered by agent 8 Says: Mehi Nol: 11, 14 Wor: 3, 12 Mehi: 33 Both agents can see: colour 27

  34. Start A speaker is chosen. My Model A hearer is chosen. A colour is chosen. Yes (P=0.001) The Speaker makes up a new word to label the colour. Decide whether speaker will be creative. No (P=0.999) The speaker says the word which they think is most likely to be a correct label for the colour based on all the examples that they have observed so far. The hearer hears the word, and remembers the corresponding colour. This example will be used to determine the word to choose, when it is the hearer’s turn to be the speaker.

  35. Evolutionary Simulations • Average lifespan (number of colour examples remembered) set at: 18, 20, 22, 24, 25, 27, 30, 35, 40, 50, 60, 70, 80, 90, 100, 110 or 120 • 25 simulation runs in each condition Languages spoken at end analysed • Only agents over half average lifespan included • Only terms for which at least 4 examples had been remembered were considered

  36. Number of Colour Terms Emerging

  37. Degree of membership in colour category  The colour term systems of four agents from a single simulation  Hue 

  38. Analyzing the Results Speakers didn’t have identical languages • Criteria needed to classify language spoken in each simulation • For each agent, terms classified as red, yellow, green, blue, purple, orange, lime, turquoise or a composite (e.g. blue-green) • Terms must be known by most adults • Classification favoured by the most agents chosen

  39. Example: One Emergent Language Denotations of Basic Color Terms for all Adults in a Community Each row is one agent Each column is a hue Boxes mark universal foci

  40. Typological Results Percentage of Color Terms of each type in the Simulations and the World Color Survey

  41. Derived Terms • 80 purple terms • 20 orange terms • 0 turquoise terms • 4 lime terms

  42. Divergence from Trajectories • 1 Blue-Red term • 1 Red-Yellow-Green term • 3 Green-Blue-Red terms Most emergent systems fitted trajectories: • 340 languages fitted trajectories • 9 contained unattested color terms • 35 had no consistent name for a universal focus • 37 had an extra term

  43. Does Increased Salience of Universal Foci Matter?

  44. Universal Foci Create More Regular Colour Term Systems • 644 purple terms • 374 orange terms • 118 lime terms • 16 turquoise terms Only 87 of 415 emergent systems fits trajectories

  45. How Reliable is WCS Data? Would a model that more closely replicated the WCS data be a better model? • Field linguists sometimes suggest that colours are much more messy than Kay et al suggest • WCS is only a sample – not a gold standard • New types of colour term systems will probably emerge if more languages are investigated

  46. Why an evolutionary model? Couldn’t we just explain everything from distances between universal foci? • How to explain which types of colour term occur together? (Purple doesn’t usually occur in a language with a green-blue composite.) • Red-yellow is commoner than yellow-green • But orange is also commoner than lime  Foci distances alone cannot explain this

  47. Explaining Composite Frequencies Red-yellow distance is 12 Yellow-green distance is 7 • So why more red-yellow than yellow-green? • Green usually forms a composite with blue • Red is too far from blue to form a composite  Only an evolutionary model could predict the effect of these interacting pressures

  48. Why a Bayesian Model? • Different assumptions in design of model would have altered learning biases • Why a Bayesian model at all? • Forces assumptions and prior bias to be explicit • Bayesian models closely parallel human learning (controversial!) Model should work just as well with an alternative learning mechanism.

  49. A Three Dimensional Model • The main limitation of the model is that it neglects the dimensions of lightness and saturation. • More accurate • Can account for pink, grey, black, white, brown • Needs a distribution over possible shapes of colour term denotations (not just size and location) • Kay and McDaniel (1978) was also 1 dimensional

  50. What does the model tell us? • Correct ordering of distances between universal foci (green and blue closest, red and blue furthest apart)? • Colour typology can be explained in terms of properties of the perceptual system and the process of linguistic transmission?  The model makes a more explicit connection between universal foci and typological data than ever before.

More Related