160 likes | 362 Views
Maternal Emotion Regulatory Strategy Mediates Maternal Depression and Child Internalizing Problems. Alysha D. Thompson, Jason J. Bendezu, Sarah E. Shea & Lisa W. Coyne. Background. 10-15% of preschoolers exhibit internalizing behaviors (Briggs-Gowan, Carter, Irwin, Wachtel, & Cicchetti, 2004).
E N D
Maternal Emotion Regulatory Strategy Mediates Maternal Depression and Child Internalizing Problems Alysha D. Thompson, Jason J. Bendezu, Sarah E. Shea & Lisa W. Coyne
Background • 10-15% of preschoolers exhibit internalizing behaviors (Briggs-Gowan, Carter, Irwin, Wachtel, & Cicchetti, 2004). • 20-50% of children and adolescents with depression have a family history of depression (Kovacs, 1997; Puig-Antich et al., 1989; Todd et al., 1993; Williamson et al., 1995). • Possible reasons for child depression: • Heritability • Environment/Situational Factors • Parenting Practices
Developmental Model of Childhood Depression Big stressors or accumulated daily hassles Vulnerability to depression, poor coping skills Sustained negative emotion = + (Kovacs et al., 2006)
Parent Emotion Regulation • Preschoolers rely on parents for: • Direct emotion regulation • Modeled strategies for coping with their negative emotion
Experiential Avoidance (EA) • Parent acceptance of emotions is important because: • May lead to parents’ direct acceptance of child emotions • May lead parents to model adaptive coping strategies • Parents may feel more in control and be able to better address child emotions
Hypotheses • Mothers reporting more experiential avoidance will be more likely to report feeling depressed and will be more likely to report internalizing problems in their children. • The relationship between maternal depression and child depressive symptoms will be better explained by maternal experiential avoidance mediating this relationship.
Participants • Participants were recruited from urban Head Starts serving diverse neighborhoods. • N = 144 mothers of Preschool children • Children ages 3-6 • Mean Mother Age = 32.01 • Median income = $17,000 • Mean number of children = 2.53
Measures • Maternal Depression: The Depression Anxiety Stress Scales, Depression Scale (DASS-21; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995) M = 4.41, SD = 6.63, Range = 0-42, α = .87 • Child Internalizing Problems: The Child Behavior Checklist, Internalizing Problems T Score (CBCL 1.5-5: Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000) M = 47.07, SD = 11.92, Range = 28-84 • Experiential Avoidance: The Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ; Hayes et al., 2004) M = 31.48, SD = 8.02, Range = 9-54, α = .58
Correlations *p < .05 **p < .01
Maternal Experiential Avoidance Maternal Depression Child Internalizing Problems EA Partially Mediates the Effect of Maternal Depression on Child Depression .24** .32** .23**(.16) Numbers in parentheses indicate standardized betas after mediation. A Sobel test found the mediated effect was statistically significant (z = 2.34, p < .05). Baron & Kenney, 1986
Discussion • Parents who are unwilling to experience their child’s negative emotions may not be able to address these emotions and thus have children who are more likely to be depressed. • Parents who are depressed are especially likely to have children who are depressed.
Strengths and Limitations • This was not a clinical population and thus it is notable to have found these relationships even in a community sample. • Data relies on self-report measures. • Depressed individuals tend to respond in a mood consistent way. • A subscale was used to measure depression rather than a full scale score. • AAQ had poor internal consistency.
Future Directions • Replicate these findings using other methods of measurement. • Use a clinical sample and look at results over time. • Specifically examine how this relates to the development of children’s emotion coping skills. • Treatment studies targeting experiential avoidance to alleviate both maternal and child depression.
References Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2000). Manual for ASEBA Preschool Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families. Baron, R. M. & Kenney, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182. Briggs-Gowan, M. J., Carter, A. S., Irwin, J. R., Wachtel, K., & Cicchetti, D. V. (2004). The brief infant–toddler social and emotional assessment: Screening for social–emotional problems and delays in competence. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 29, 143−155. Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., Wilson, K. G., Bissett, R. T., Pistorello, J., & Toarmino, D. (2004). Measuring experiential avoidance: A preliminary test of a working model. The Psychological Record, 54, 553-578. Kovacs, M., Devlin, B., Pollock, M., Richards, C., & Mukerji, P. (1997). A controlled family history study of childhood-onset depressive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 613–623. Kovacs, M., Sherrill, J., George, C. J., Pollock, M., Tumuluru, R. V., & Ho, V. (2006). Contextual-emotion regulation therapy for childhood depression: Description and pilot testing of a new intervention. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 892-903. Lovibond, P. F., Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional stress: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behavior Research and Therapy, 33, 335-343. Puig-Antich, J., Goetz, D., Davies, M., Kaplan, T., Davies, S., Ostrow, L., Asnis, L., Twomey, J., Iyengar, S., & Ryan, N. D. (1989). A controlled family history study of prepubertal major depressive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 406–418. Williamson, D. E., Ryan, N. D., Birmaher, B., Dahl, R. E., Kaufman, J., Rao, U., & Puig-Antich, J. (1995). A case-control family history study of depression in adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 1596–1607.