370 likes | 442 Views
New Complications. Adding a control group to our design deals with the confounds discussed so far Unfortunately, however, it also creates new potential confounds. Selection. With a between-subjects variable, one has multiple groups of people
E N D
New Complications • Adding a control group to our design deals with the confounds discussed so far • Unfortunately, however, it also creates new potential confounds
Selection • With a between-subjects variable, one has multiple groups of people • You want the groups of people to be as similar as possible to one another • This is known as having equivalent groups • Notice that I did not say identical groups • The groups will differ to some extent
Group Differences • What differences are acceptable or unacceptable? • Acceptable • The groups differ in terms of an uncontrolled variable that does NOT correlate with the DV • Unacceptable • The groups differ in terms of an uncontrolled variable that CORRELATES with the DV • These are known as systematic differences
Example • Does high vs. low waitperson attentiveness affect tipping? • Acceptable • Groups differ in terms of hair color • High attentiveness = mostly blondes • Low attentiveness = mostly brunettes • Unacceptable • Groups differ in terms of table waiting experience • High attentiveness = mostly waited tables • Low attentiveness = mostly not
Confound • If groups differ in terms of an uncontrolled variable that correlates with the DV, then the study MAY be confounded • To determine whether the study is confounded, one must consider whether the uncontrolled variable provides an alternative explanation for the study’s outcome
Example • Outcome • Tips were higher in the high attentiveness condition than the low attentiveness condition • Not confounded • High attentiveness = mostly not • Low attentiveness = mostly waited tables • Confounded • High attentiveness = mostly waited tables • Low attentiveness = mostly not
Example • Outcome • Tips were equal in the high and low attentiveness conditions • Not confounded • High attentiveness = mostly waited tables • Low attentiveness = mostly not • Confounded • High attentiveness = mostly not • Low attentiveness = mostly waited tables
Solutions • There are a number of ways to mitigate a Selection confound • Make the uncontrolled variable another IV • Create equivalent groups • By randomly placing participants into one’s conditions • This is better than matching when collecting a large sample • By matching participants in one’s conditions • This is better than randomization when collecting a small sample
Example: Make IV • Do waitperson attentiveness (high vs. low) and parton experience (waited tables vs. not) affect tipping?
Example: Randomize • Randomly assign participants to the high and low attentiveness conditions • One can do this purely randomly wherein every participant has an equal chance of being assigned to either condition • One can do this quasi-randomly with the constraint that once a participant has been assigned to a condition no other participants will be assigned to that condition until all conditions have an equal number of participants • This is known as block randomization
Example: Matching • Pair participants who have waited tables • Pair participants who have not waited tables • Randomly assign one member of each pair to the high and low attentiveness conditions • To be successful, one must have a good reason to match participants, as well as a good way to measure the matching variable
Our studies • Experimental group • Pre-Test, Verbal Training w/ Feedback, Post-Test • Control group • Pre-Test, Verbal Training w/o Feedback, Post-Test • Participants were placed randomly into the Experimental (Feedback) and Control (No Feedback) groups
Attrition • Sometimes people in one group quit the study more often than people in the other group • This is known as an attrition problem
Attrition 1. Collect active Pre-Test data 2a. Experimental group: Provide training with feedback • Many people in this group do not complete the study 2b. Control group: Provide training without feedback • Few people in this group do not complete the study 3. Don’t change anything else 4. Collect active Post-Test data 5. Compare Pre and Post-Test data • Is difference due to full training, or who quit the study?
Solution • There is nothing methodological that one can do to prevent an attrition confound • One must be watchful for them, and adjust conclusions accordingly, if one is suspected
Order • Within-Subjects variables expose each participant to every level of the Independent Variable • Accordingly, in most circumstances, the order in which the levels are presented to the participant becomes an issue • This is known as an Order problem
Solution • Order problems can be mitigated by presenting the levels of the Independent Variable in different orders to different people • The orders must be created so that each level occurs (roughly) equally often at each phase of the testing session
Example • Consider a variation of our studies • In this variation, participants will throw the beanbag to a target, with their eyes closed, and with them open • Throwing repeatedly might cause fatigue, so throwing in second half of the study will always be worse than throwing earlier in the study • Solution • Half of the participants throw with their eyes closed first, and then with their eyes open, while the other half throws in the opposite order
Counterbalancing • Complete counterbalancing • Uses each of the possible orders of treatments • The number of possible orders is found by X! • where “X” is the number of conditions and “!” is the factorial • As the number of conditions increases, then the number of potential orders increases markedly • Requires as many participants as possible orders
Counterbalancing • Partial counterbalancing • Uses a subset of the possible orders • The subset may • Be chosen at random • Employ a Latin Square
Latin Square • An example of a Latin Square would be • A, B, C, D • B, C, D, A • C, D, A, B • D, A, B, C • In a standard latin square, each level occurs equally often in every sequential position. • One or more people would be exposed to each of these orders
Our Independent Variables • Between-Subjects • Feedback • Two conditions, i.e., with and without feedback • Within-Subjects • Session • Three levels, i.e., Pre-Test, Training, Post-Test
Our studies • In our studies, we must run the levels of our Within-Subjects variable (Session) in a particular order, so we must not counterbalance
Carry-Over Effects • When employing a within-subjects IV, what happens during one level sometimes affects what happens during subsequent levels • This is known as a carry-over effect
Example • Does high vs. low waitperson attentiveness affect tipping? • High attentiveness Low attentiveness • In this order, Low attentiveness seems worse than it would have otherwise because of prior exposure to High attentiveness • Low attentiveness High attentiveness • In this order, High attentiveness seems better than it would have otherwise because of prior exposure to Low attentiveness
Example • Note that each sequence has a unique effect on the data • High Low = Low seems worse • Low High = High seems better • Note also that these effects do NOT cancel • The worsening of the Low attentiveness level is not counteracted by anything • The enhancing of the High attentiveness level is not counteracted by anything
Counterbalancing • Given that each sequence has a unique effect on the data, and those unique effects do not cancel one another, counterbalancing does NOT eliminate carry-over effects
Example • High Low = Low seems worse • Low High = High seems better • When you combine the data, you get • High + High (better) • Low + Low (worse)
Solution • If one suspects that a carry-over effect may happen, then it is advisable to use a Between-Subjects Independent Variable • This has consequences, though, because within-subjects variables require less participants and have greater statistical power than between-subjects variables
Bias • Lastly, two forms of bias can confound one’s experiment • Experimenter bias • Experimenters influence the participants unintentionally in ways that make the study come out as expected • Participant bias • Participants may act differently because they are in an experiment, which is known as a Hawthorne effect • Participants may try to be too good, which is known as an Orne effect
Solutions • Experimenter bias • Minimize and standardize the interaction between the participant and the experimenter • When possible, design studies so that participants and/or experimenters and participants do not know what condition is being run • These are known as blind, and double-blind studies, respectively
Solutions • Participant bias • Minimize and standardize the interaction between the participant and the experimenter • Provide the participants with just enough information so that they can make an educated judgment about informed consent, but no more • This is known as having naïve participants
Our studies • In our studies, we use a standard consent form and instructions, in order to minimize and standardize the experimenters’ interaction with the participants