1 / 33

Behavioral paradigm development for fMRI and EEG Jason Zevin Sackler Institute

Behavioral paradigm development for fMRI and EEG Jason Zevin Sackler Institute. What’s your problem?. What’s your problem? Clinical: “Does this treatment alleviate a particular symptom?” Translational: “Is activity in this region related to some feature of a disorder/disease?”

steffi
Download Presentation

Behavioral paradigm development for fMRI and EEG Jason Zevin Sackler Institute

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Behavioral paradigm development for fMRI and EEG Jason Zevin Sackler Institute

  2. What’s your problem?

  3. What’s your problem? Clinical: “Does this treatment alleviate a particular symptom?” Translational: “Is activity in this region related to some feature of a disorder/disease?” Basic: “How does the brain accomplish some function?”

  4. What’s your problem? Are you interested in a particular region or network? Are you interested in a particular behavior or function? Are you interested in a particular population? Do you care more about spatial or temporal resolution?

  5. Different approaches have different strengths/weaknesses, and are suited to different kinds of problems. Electrophysiology (EEG) - high temporal resolution - low spatial resolution Analysis approaches - event related potentials (ERPs) - topographic/source analysis - continuous EEG

  6. Different approaches have different strengths/weaknesses, and are suited to different kinds of problems. fMRI - low temporal resolution - high spatial resolution Analysis approaches - block designs - event-related designs - correlation analyses - fancy stuff we won’t have time for

  7. EEG

  8. 256-Channel Geodesic Sensor Net

  9. EEG Signals

  10. ERPs EEG is averaged Time locked to a stimulus event May also be averaged to a response event (Response Potential) Increases signal-to-noise

  11. Age 6.5 220 ms Age 8.3220 ms Age 26150 ms Words Symbols Difference -7/14.0 µV 0 µV 7/14.0 µV < < p<0.01 p<0.01 -11.4 0t 11.4 Development Print Processingin the first 200 milliseconds Maurer et al. (2007)

  12. What’s actually being measured: Current related to local field potentials in cortex Measurements are taken at the scalp, though…

  13. Locating activity can be tricky! Orientation plays a role in what you see at the scalp… Graphics from http://www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/EEG/img/Physiological_basis_EEG.gif http://ww2.heartandstroke.ca/images/english/english_brain.jpg

  14. Given a dipole source inside the head, we can solve for what it would look like on the scalp. But the pattern of activity at the scalp has a one-to-many mapping back onto possible dipole sources. The Inverse problem

  15. Alpha - strong in relaxed, awake states. Theta - may be largely driven by hippocampus, prominent in short term memory tasks So, what’s the upside? A more “direct” measure of neural activity. Lets you look at neat stuff like oscillation frequency: Figures from Wikipedia entry on EEG, of all places

  16. Figure from: Klimesch, W. (1999) EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: A review and analysis, Brain Research Reviews, Volume 29,169-195.

  17. Temporal resolution allows fine-grained inferences about the timing of neural events. Molholm, S., Ritter, W., Murray, M.M., Javitt, D.C., Schroeder,C.E., and Foxe, J.J. (2002) Multisensory auditory-visual interactions during early sensory processing in humans: a high-density electrical mapping study, Cognitive Brain Research, 14, 115-128.

  18. Some considerations for designing EEG studies: How important is spatial resolution? Maybe the process you care about is related to a general “brain state,” e.g. a stage of sleep.

  19. Some considerations for designing EEG studies: Can you get enough data to do ERPs? (typically ~100 trials) Interestingly, kids and infants, with their thin skulls and little heads, give better EEG data and need fewer trials (but they wiggle around more).

  20. Some considerations for designing EEG studies: How important is temporal resolution? Here, the argument that multisensory integration happens early depends on rapid responses very short stimuli. But what if your stimuli are naturally long, or vary in duration?

  21. fMRI

  22. I’ll skip over the basics, because they’ve been covered earlier. But let’s think about time.

  23. Block designs can maximize power. response to a single brief stimulus activation intervals summed up HRFs from activations (adapted from the afni regression tutorial)

  24. Why would you ever sacrifice power?

  25. (audience participation)

  26. Slow event-related designs Stimulus (“Neural”) HRF Predicted Data  = You can recover the HRF nicely, but you don’t get much data. http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/

  27. = Fast event related designs without “jitter” Stimulus (“Neural”) HRF Predicted Data Lots of data, but no idea where it’s coming from in time. http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/

  28. = Let’s get (sort of) random. Stimulus (“Neural”) HRF Predicted Data A little randomness in the timing of events permits better recovery of hemodynamic responses, with relatively rapid events. Still nowhere close to the temporal resolution of EEG. http://www.columbia.edu/cu/psychology/tor/

  29. You don’t have to get super fancy, though, to see interesting things. In this study, Singer et al. administered shocks to women and their partners. Even a slow, event related design shows interesting overlap between getting shocked and watching your honey get shocked. They also took measures of empathy using paper and pencil outside the scanner.

  30. These correlations are probably bogus (we can talk about it if there’s time). BUT, the principle of measuring some behavioral trait and relating it to brain responses is sound. To do a study like this you probably want a very powerful design (or stimulus) in order to be sure you’re driving activity in the regions of interest.

  31. But you might want to do something subtler with timing… Here, the authors (including next week’s lecturer) wanted to measure the effect of context on responses to ambiguous faces. They used temporal jitter to separate activity due to the context out from activity related to seeing the face.

  32. Some considerations for designing fMRI studies: Do you want “power” or “subtlety?” - how much time do you have to collect data? (kids, patients sometimes kinda hate being in the scanner) - are you trying to characterize the function of a region/network, or relate activity in some well-characterized region to a population variable? Does dealing with the timing of stimulus presentation to get a nice HRF make your experiment awkward? Slow? What’s the impact on behavior? (This happens in EEG, too.)

More Related