E N D
1. Ethics, informed consent and statistics Paul S. Mueller, MD, MPH, FACP
Division of General Internal Medicine
Mayo Clinic Rochester
2. Questions we will cover today What are the elements of informed consent?
Do people perceive risk similarly? If not, why not?
My test is “positive” (“negative”). What does that mean?
The treatment prevents (cures, etc.) a disease by 50%. Is it a good treatment?
3. "There are lies, damned lies and statistics."
4. Informed consent Underlying ethical principle: respect for patient autonomy
Elements of informed consent:
Information
Patient voluntarily agrees with plan
Patient has decision-making capacity
5. AMA code on informed consent2000-2001 (8.08)
6. Informed consent: legal aspects Based upon negligence principles
State law governs malpractice
Differing state standards shaped by case law:
Professional practice standard: customary for other clinicians to do
Reasonable person standard: what a reasonable person needs to know (most states)
7. Information: what would a reasonable patient want to know? Nature of the intervention
Benefits of intervention
Risks
Alternatives (and their benefits and risks)
8. Risk What is risk?
Webster’s (1999): “ The chance of injury, damage, or loss; dangerous chance; hazard”
Understanding risk is complex:
Objective: quantitative; patients and clinicians have limited comprehension of the quantitative aspects of risk
Subjective: how important is it to the patient?
11. Situations during which risk is commonly discussed with patients Diagnostic tests:
“If test X is positive, the chance of disease Y is…”
Treatments:
“The chance disease Y is cured by treatment X is...”
“Treatment X reduces the risk (or recurrence) of disease Y by…”
12. If the test is positive, do I have the disease?
14. The chance a patient with a positive test has the disease is: True positives ? (true positives + false positives)
Here: 24 ? (24 + 14) = 63%
This is known as positive predictive value; what we usually want to know! Similar concept: negative predictive value (probability patient doesn’t have disease if test is negative)
Here: 56 ? (56 + 6) = 90%
16. The chance a test will be positive if the patient has the disease is: True positives ? (true positives + false negatives)
Here: 24 ? (24 + 6) = 80%
This is known as sensitivity
Measures the effectiveness of a test Similar concept: specificity (probability test is negative if the patient doesn’t have the disease)
Here: 56 ? (56 + 14) = 80%
18. Real example: screening mammography For women with no FH of breast cancer:
Sensitivity: 70-88%
Specificity: 89-91%
Positive predictive value: 1-6%
Sensitivity and specificity increase with age
Mammography is not a perfect screening test for breast cancer
19. The bottom line Unfortunately, no test is perfect
When a test is positive, the chance the patient has the disease is almost never 100%
Not all patients with positive tests have the disease the test is intended to detect
20. When a test is negative, and the disease is still suspected, what do clinicians usually do next?
21. How do I know if the treatment that my doctor suggests is good?
22. Hierarchy of evidence
24. Worth noting: Almost every trial demonstrates:
Some people get better on their own without treatment
Not all people who are treated get better
25. By how much is treatment new better than treatment old? Absolute risk reduction = risk old treatment - risk new
Here: 60% - 30% = 30%
“The risk of the bad outcome on treatment new is 30% less than on treatment old.” Relative risk = (risk old treatment - risk new) ? risk old
Here: (60% - 30%) ? 60% = .50
“On treatment new, the chance of the bad outcome is 50% the risk of being on treatment old.”
26. Expressed as graph
27. What is the problem with relative risk?
28. For each scenario, relative risk reduction is 50%, but the absolute risk reduction is much different
31. Physician’s Health StudyNew Engl J Med 1989;321:129-135 N = 22,071
11,037 received aspirin (ASA) and 11,034 placebo
Incidence (risk) of MI in ASA group was 255/100,000 per year or 0.26% per year Incidence (risk) in placebo group was 440/100,000 or 0.44% per year
Absolute risk reduction of MI with ASA: 0.18% per year
Relative risk reduction: 44%
33. “4S” TrialLancet 1994;344:1383-1389
36. Be careful when contemplating risk What are the characteristics of the patients enrolled in the study?
Which risk? Absolute or relative?
Physicians, the media, medical journals, industry and lawyers often talk in terms of relative risk
However, absolute risk reduction is often more relevant to patients
37. Number needed to treat (NNT): a better way of communicating risk? NNT = number of persons needed to treat to prevent (cure, etc.) one case
NNT = 1/absolute risk reduction
Physicians Health Study: NNT = 1/0.0018 = 556 Physicians Health Study: would have to treat 556 patients with ASA to prevent one MI
4S trial: NNT = 1/0.035 = 29; would have to treat 29 patients to prevent one coronary death
39. Conclusions Medical statistics can be are complex and can be confusing
No test is perfect
Results of trials are often presented in terms of relative risk, which may be irrelevant to patients
Effective communication of risk is essential for informed decision-making
40. Acknowledgements Amit K. Ghosh, MD, FACP
BMJ 9/27/2003; a large portion of this issue is devoted to communicating risk to patients