440 likes | 628 Views
Interesting Statistical Phenomenon. VA San Diego Addictions Seminar 4/16/08 Kevin Cummins. Definitions. Paradox : a statement that is seemingly contradictory or opposed to common sense and yet is perhaps true Fallacy : a false or mistaken idea
E N D
Interesting Statistical Phenomenon VA San DiegoAddictions Seminar4/16/08Kevin Cummins
Definitions • Paradox: a statement that is seemingly contradictory or opposed to common sense and yet is perhaps true • Fallacy: a false or mistaken idea • Principle: a comprehensive and fundamental law, doctrine, or assumption
Outline • Objective • Simpson’s Paradox • Will Roger’s Paradox • Lord’s Paradox • Berkson’s Paradox • Monte Hall Paradox • Others
Objective • Create awareness of several statistical issues that might arise during your research
Outline • Objective • Simpson’s Paradox • Will Roger’s Paradox • Lord’s Paradox • Berkson’s Paradox • Monte Hall Paradox • Others
Simpson’s Paradox Occurs when the relationship between two categorical variables is reversed after a third variables is introduced. The relationship between two variables differs within subgroups compared to that observed for the aggregated data.
America West Alaska Airlines .29 .17 .14 .11 .08 .05
Simpson’s Paradox: Remedies/Responses Study DesignUse ExperimentsCollect appropriate covariate data Know the Research System Collect appropriate covariate data Analytically introduce conditionals(i.e. moderators/covariates) Use appropriate interpretations
Outline • Objective • Simpson’s Fallacy • Will Roger’s Paradox • Lord’s Paradox • Berkson’s Paradox • Monte Hall Paradox • Others
Will Roger’s Paradox “When the Okies left Oklahoma and moved to California, they raised the average intelligence level in both states.”
The Will Rogers Paradox (WRP) is obtained when moving an element from one set to another set the average values of both sets increase: The effect will occur when both of these conditions are met: 1. The element being moved is below average for its current set. 2. The element being moved is above the current average of the set it is entering.
WRP: Health Insurance Example PPO No Longer Free The 1997 migration moved lower cost PPO subjects into the HMO Young et al. 1999
Will Rogers: Remedies/Responses Know Your System In This Case: Statistically Adjust for Baseline Costs
Outline • Objective • Simpson’s Fallacy • Will Roger’s Paradox • Lord’s Paradox • Berkson’s Paradox • Monte Hall Paradox • Others
Lord’s Paradox • Situation where change score analysis and ANCOVA yield apparently conflicting results
A Simplified Example • Assessment of a supplemental educational program (tutoring) • 10 students from different schools • 5 schools opted into the programs (free-choice) • Pre and post assessments given • No random/sampling/measurement error (simplified)
Statistician One Calculates difference scores for each group Change scores the same for both groups Statistician Two Adjust for initial score Finds group differences Two Statisticians
Paired t-Test Statistician One Data: group 1 vs. group 2 t = -0.002, df = 299, p-value = 0.99 ANCOVA Statistician Two Coefficients: Value Pr(>|t|) (Intercept) 15.0 0.00 Pre 0.5 0.00 Group 20.0 0.00 Two Statisticians
Statistician’s Assumptions • Statistician one assumes that in the absence of any differential treatment effect the two groups despite different baselines would show equivalent changes • Statistician two assumes that in the absence of any differential treatment effect the change of the groups as a whole is the same as the change within groups • Both of these causal assumptions are untestable
Establish causal/system assumptions Use the best descriptive statement Use and report multiple approaches (Wright 2006) Know that ANCOVA has greater power Graph your data Lord’s Paradox: Remedies/Responses
Outline • Objective • Simpson’s Fallacy • Will Roger’s Paradox • Lord’s Principle • Berkson’s Paradox • Monte Hall Paradox • Others
Berkson’s Paradox An association reported from a hospital-based case-control study can be distorted If cases and controls experience differential hospital admission rates with respect to the suspected causal factor
Typical Berkson Example Example from Roberts et al. 1978 Investigated the relationship between circulatory and respiratory disease. Sampled the general population and hospital populations.
Circulatory Disease OR = 3.9 [95% CI: 1.4-10.9]
Circulatory Disease OR = 1.3 [95% CI: 0.9-2.3]
Real Berkson Example Example from Berkson 1946 Hypothetical example exploring the relationship between diabetes and cholecystitis No greater admission rate for subjects with multiple conditions Different rates of admission for cases and controls
Berkson’s: Remedies/Responses • There is no analytical mitigation • Limit conclusions • Consider alternative study design
Outline • Objective • Simpson’s Fallacy • Will Roger’s Paradox • Lord’s Principle • Berkson’s Paradox • Monte Hall Paradox • Others
Lindley’s Paradox • Standard Sampling Theory VS. Bayesian Theory Under some circumstances strong evidence against the null hypothesis doesn’t result in the null being rejected
Benford’s Law Ones are the most common leading digit in most data. Notice that if a data entry (base 10) begins with a 1, the entry has to be at most doubled to have a first significant digit of 2. However, if a data entry begins with a 9, it only has to be increased by, at most, 11% to change the first significant digit into a 1.
Review Big PictureUse care to interpret observational studiesKnow your system Conditional ResponsesSimpson’sLord’sWill Roger’s Perspective ProblemsBerkson’sMonte Hall