210 likes | 325 Views
From Big Five to Big One: Higher-order structural hierarchy of personality. Janek Musek University of Ljubljana SLOVENIA. Outline of presentation. Introduction: New proposal of personality structure Two studies Methods Results General factor of personality (GFP)
E N D
From Big Five to Big One: Higher-order structural hierarchy of personality Janek Musek University of Ljubljana SLOVENIA ALPS ADRIA PSYCHOLOGY CONFERENCE LJUBLJANA 2008
Outline of presentation • Introduction: New proposal of personality structure • Two studies • Methods • Results • General factor of personality (GFP) • Cross-cultural stability of GFP • Psychological meaning of GFP • Conclusions
Models of personality structure • A great variety of structural models in psychology • Intelligence • Personality (outside cognitive abilities domain) • Other domains (e.g. well-being, values…) • Debate in personality structure domain • 16 (Cattell), 5 (Big Five), 3 (Eysenck) or 2 (Digman)? • Question of possible general factor of personality largely ignored • Notable exceptions (Saucier & Goldberg, 2003; Stankov, 2005) • Yet… • The evidence for GFP in Five Factor Model
GFP considered seriously • Strategic correlations among B5 • Typical example below (-N, A, C, E; O, E) • These correlations cannot be reduced to the evaluation, social desirability or methodological artifacts • On the other hand, GFP correlates very substantially with psychologically meaningful variables like self-esteem, self-concept, well-being, emotionality (positive, negative affect), motivation (approach-avoidance) and others even if partialized for social desirability • Support from evolutionary oriented research (Rushton, Figuereido)
Method: Study I • Research design: multivariate (exploratory and confirmatory FA in first line; other multivariate analyses as well) • Three samples in first research: • Sample 1: N=301 (120 F, 181 M; mean age=36.95; SD=10.37) • Sample 2: N=185 (100 F, 85 M; mean age=39.11; SD=13.26) • Sample 3: N=285 (165 F, 120 M; mean age=16.37; SD=1.24) • More samples and subsamples (Slovenian and other) in further investigations (not published yet) • Measures (all Slovenian translated versions): • BFI (John, 1990; John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991; John & Srivastava, 1999) • IPIP-300 (Goldberg, 1999) • BFO (Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Borgogni, L. & Perugini, M., 1993; Caprara, Barbaranelli & Borgogni, 1994) • Data from other studies supported the strength of first factor and consequently yielded GFP (practically all available results including cross-cultural studies with nationally aggregated data /Musek, 2008/; also data collected by Rushton and colleagues, 2008)
Exploratory results • Results for scales BFI, IPIP, BFO), facets (IPIP) and items (BFI, BFO) • Factorizability of BF scales and items • Direct extraction of one single factor • GFP explained • 50,20% of variance in 5 BFI scales • 23,58% of variance in 44 BFI items • 40,18% of variance in 5 IPIP scales • 25,13% of variance in 30 IPIP facets • 44,84% of variance in BFO SCALES • Practically identical factors in scale and non-scale solutions
Confirmatory results • All selected indices confirmed the underlying models
Method: Study II • How stable or universal is GFP? • 5 different studies on culturally different samples (omitted for the sake of space, yet focused on the following) • Aggregated data • On 56 national samples from the study of Schmitt et al. (2006) • Instruments: • BFI, NEO-FFI, NEO-PI
56 nations data (Schmitt et al., 2006) • Correlations • Factorizability (acceptable; KMO=0,655) • Suggested 1 factor extraction (all indices including Kaiser criterion, scree test and parallel test)
56 nations data (Schmitt et al., 2006), cont. • Scree plot • Factor loadings • Confirmed meaning of GFP in B5 terms: high versus low conscientiousness (C), agreeableness (A), stability (-N), extraversion (E) and openness (O)
Main issues for discussion • Psychological nature of GFP • Connections with other major psychological variables • Possible biological bases of GFP • Evolutionary • Genetic • Neurophysiological
The meaning of GFP in terms of the Big Five • Irrespective of the perspective of data (within or across cultural milieus) • High versus low emotional stability (-N), conscientiousness (C), agreeableness (A), extraversion (E) and openness (O) • GFP formula: • -N,+C,+A,+E,+O
GFP, emotionality, well-being and self-esteem • Substantial and relatively high association • About 60% common variance between GFP and these measures Table 7. Correlations and squared multiple correlation between GFPs and the measures of emotionality, well-being, and self-esteem *P<0.05, **P<0.01 (two-tailed) a GFP obtained by direct extraction of single factor. b GFP obtained by stepwise hierarchical higher-order factoring.
Correlations (N=301) • All correlations are significant • The highest correlations between general factors of personality and well-being domain (including self-esteem)
Psychological meaning of GFP • Evaluation and social desirability certainly contribute to the correlations among items and scales of B5 measures • Some other factors can also affect the correlations between items • Correlations between lexically short expressions (e.g. adjectives) are bigger than correlations between lalrger contextual questions or statements • Faking tendency increases the correlations (Ziegler, 2006) • Yet all these factors do not reduce the correlations essentially • For instance in our investigations (N=108) the social desirability (SDS) explained about 18 percent of GFP, and that is much less than general well-being (gFB; see the regression model below) • It seems that GFP has a definite psychological content, which is strongly associated with well-being and is also remarkably heritable (Musek, 2007; Rushton, 2008)
More speculations about the nature of GFP • Probable existence of a common dimensions unifying basic dimensions of personality, emotionality, motivation, psychological or subjective well-being and self-esteem • Evolutionary, genetic and neurophysiological basis of that dimension • GFP is a personality representative of it
Evolutionary and genetic aspects of GFP • Evolutionary advantages of those personal, emotional and motivational characteristics that are more prone to the social approval and more promoting better well-being • Moving toward the big equation in psychology, at least in personality domain?
GFP correlates in personality, emotionality, motivation, self, well-being, culture, and biology (Musek, 2007)
Some important references • Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529–550. • Becker, P. (1999). Beyond the Big Five. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 511–530. • Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Larsen, J. T., Poehlmann, K. M.,& Ito, T. A. (2000). The psychophysiology of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 173–191). New York: Guilford Press. • Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 668-678. • Carver, C. S., Sutton, S. K.,& Scheier, M. F. (2000). Action, emotion, and personality: Emerging conceptual integration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 741-751. • Carver, C. S.,& White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319-333. • DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2001). Higher-order factors of the big five predict conformity: are there neuroses of health? Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 533-552. • Diener, E. (1996). Traits can be powerful, but are not enough--Lessons from subjective well-being. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 389-399. • Diener, E. (1998). Subjective well-being and personality. In D. Barone, M. Hersen, & V. Van Hasselt (Eds.), Advanced personality, (pp. 311-334). New York: Plenum Press. • Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (1999a). Temperament, personality, and subjective well-being. In Kahneman, D., Diener, E. & Schwarz, N. (Eds.) Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 213-229). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. • Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (1999b). Personality, and subjective well-being. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.). Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology ( pp. 213-229). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. • Diener, E., Smith, & Fujita, F. (1995). The personality structure of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1, 130-141. • Digman, J. M. (1997). Higher-order factors of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1246-1256. • Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Approach-Avoidance Motivation in Personality: Approach and Avoidance Temperaments and Goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 82, No. 5, 804-818. • Figueredo, A. J., Vásquez, G., Brumbach, B. H., & Schneider, S. M. R. (2004). The heritability of life history strategy: The K-factor, covitality, and personality. • Social Biology, 51, 121–143. • Figueredo, A. J., Vásquez, G., Brumbach, B. H., & Schneider, S. M. R. (2007). The K-factor, covitality, and personality: A psychometric test of life history theory. • Human Nature, 18, 47–73. • Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T.,& Elliot, A. J. (2000). Behavioral activation and inhibition in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1135-1149. • Panksepp, J. (1999). Affective neuroscience. Oxford University Press. • Assessment, 5, 164-172. • Reich, J. W., Zautra, A. J. & Davis, M. (2003). Dimensions of Affect Relationships: Models and Their Integrative Implications. Review of General Psychology, Vol. 7, No. 1, 66-83. • Rushton, J. P., Bons, T. A., & Hur, Y. M. (2008). The genetics and evolution of the General factor of personality. Journal of Research in Personality, In Press. March 3, 2008. • Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. R. (2003). The structure of personality attributes. In M. R. Barrick & A. M. Ryan (Eds.), Personality and work (pp. 1-29). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. • Schimmack, U., & Diener, E. (2003). Predictive validity of explicit and implicit self-esteem for subjective well-being. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 100-106. • Spoont, M. R. (1992). Modulatory role of serotonin in neural information processing, implications for human psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 330–350 • Stankov, L. (2005). g Factor. Issues of design and interpretation. In O. Wilhelm & R. W.Engle (Eds.), Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence (pp. 279-293). Thousand Oaks, Ca., London, New Delhi: Sage Publications. • Tork, I. (1990). Anatomy of the serotonergic system. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 600, 9–35. • Watson, D. & Clark, L. A. (1992). Affects separable and inseparable: On the hierarchical arrangement of the negative affects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 489-505. • Yik, M. S. M., & Russell, J. A. (2001). Predicting the Big Two of Affect from the Big Five of Personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 247–277.