1 / 40

Implicit imitation of regional dialects

Implicit imitation of regional dialects. Sara C. Phillips First Qualifying Paper Presentation 5-23-11. slides (with references) available at: http://www.ling.ohio-state.edu/~phillips email: phillips@ling.osu.edu. Acknowledgements.

conley
Download Presentation

Implicit imitation of regional dialects

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Implicit imitation of regional dialects Sara C. Phillips First Qualifying Paper Presentation 5-23-11 slides (with references) available at: http://www.ling.ohio-state.edu/~phillips email: phillips@ling.osu.edu

  2. Acknowledgements • Cynthia G. Clopper, Shari R. Speer, Kathryn Campbell-Kibler, Mark Pitt, Jane Stuart-Smith • Lauren Eskin, Joselyn Gilbert, and Kenney Hensley for help in data collection and annotation • Speer Lab, Phonies, Changelings, SoMean, Proseminar,Cogfest 2010, MCWOP 2010

  3. Imitation People imitate one another’s speech: • gestures, speech rate, f0 (Giles et al. 1991, Jungers et al. 2002) • VOT (Shockley et al., 2004; Nielsen, 2011) • vowel quality (Babel, 2009; Tilsen, 2009) • Interactive (“accommodation”) • Non-interactive (“phonetic imitation”)

  4. Phonetic Imitation: Experience • Imitation in word shadowing increases with exposure to the stimulus materials (Goldinger 1998). • Shadowers imitate stimulus recordings more if they approximate the phonotactic structure of their native language (Nye and Fowler 2003). • Imitation doesn’t happen across the board (Babel 2009). • Some vowels (/a/, /æ/) are imitated more than others. • Speakers stay within pre-existing production inventory while imitating.

  5. Research Questions • What is the effect of regional dialect on imitation? • Is real life experience like in-lab exposure? • More imitation of familiar dialects? • Do shadowers imitate talker-specific or dialect-specific properties? • Salient dialect features (e.g. vowel quality) imitated more than other features (e.g. duration, f0)? (Pierrehumbert 2002)

  6. Shadowing Task: Motivation • Non-interactive, socially impoverished task • if real life dialect familiarity is similar to lab experience, shadowers should imitate familiar dialects more than unfamiliar dialects • should have more exemplars similar to familiar dialect targets, thus more ability to produce close imitation

  7. Shadowing Task: Participants 31 monolingual American English speakers 3 groups according to dialect: “Midland”: 15 [9M/6F] (e.g. Columbus, OH) “Northern”: 8 [3M/5F] (e.g. Cleveland, OH) “Mobile”: 8 [2M/6F] (e.g. Mid and South)

  8. Dialect Regions Northern Midland

  9. Shadowing Task: Stimuli • 55 monosyllabic CVC English words • from the Indiana Speech Project corpus (Clopper et al., 2002) • produced by each of 6 female talkers from two dialect regions : 3 “Midland” talkers 3 “Northern” talkers Total : 330 targets

  10. Shadowing Task: Procedure Word-shadowing task (Goldinger, 1998) • Part 1 (baseline) : read all 55 words aloud • Part 2 (shadowing) : hear all 330 targets and repeat aloud as quickly and naturally as possible • implicit imitation – not told to imitate • No blocking by target dialect region or talker, random presentation order

  11. Shadowing Task: Measurements • vowel quality (F1, F2) at vowel midpoint • duration (vowel, onset, coda) • f0 at vowel midpoint • f0 trajectory (change in f0) • response time

  12. Shadowing Task: Predictions • More imitation of tokens from familiar dialect region than unfamiliar dialect region • Interaction between shadower dialect and target dialect (Mid-North < Mid-Mid) • Northern shadowers may be familiar with both target dialects • More imitation of salient dialect features? • North /ɛ/, /æ/ • Midland /u/

  13. Shadowing Task: Results • No significant interactions between shadower dialect and target dialect • Effects of shadower dialect: • midpoint f0: Northern < Midland (t = -2.3, p < .05) • vowel duration: Mobile and Northern > Midland (t = 2.5, p < .05; t = 4.1, p < .001) • More imitation of longer (t = 11.6, p < .001), higher f0 (t = 2.1, p < .05) targets

  14. AXB Task: Perceived Imitation • baseline, original target, repetition  A, X, B • 6 shadowers: 2 Midland, 2 North, 2 Mobile • 60 listeners (10 per shadower) • Question: Did shadowers imitate dialect-specific variation in addition to talker-specific variation?

  15. AXB Task AX B baseline target repetition “Which one, A or B, sounds more similar to, or like a better imitation of, X?”

  16. AXB Task AX B repetition target baseline “Which one, A or B, sounds more similar to, or like a better imitation of, X?”

  17. AXB Task: Dialect or Talker? 3 conditions: 1. same talker X = original talker from shadowing task 2. different talker X = different talker, same dialect as original 3. different dialect X = different dialect, different talker from original same talker different talker different dialect

  18. AXB Task: Dialect or Speaker? Imitation only of talker: same talker imitated more than different talkerdifferent dialect same talker target talker shadower shadower different talker shadower shadower shadower different dialect talker dialect both

  19. AXB Task: Dialect or Speaker? Imitation of dialect and talker: same talker, different talker imitated more than different dialect same talker target talker shadower shadower different talker shadower shadower shadower different dialect talker dialect both

  20. AXB Results: Condition • listeners were sensitive to dialect mismatch • shadowers may have imitated dialect properties

  21. AXB: Dialect Markers? • Acoustic correlates with AXB scores

  22. AXB: Dialect Markers? • Acoustic correlates with AXB scores not dialect markers …but in our stimuli, Northern targets were generally longer than Midland targets (t = 12.8, p < .0001) could create illusion of dialect imitation

  23. Conclusions • No evidence that shadowers imitated familiar dialects more than unfamiliar dialects • AXB perception results: • imitation of talker-specific variation • illusion (?) of dialect imitation • Only very small degree of imitation

  24. Future directions • Replicate with… • all low frequency words, which are likely to be imitated (Goldinger 1998) • dialects that are less mutually familiar e.g. North, Midland and South • focus on vowels that differentiate dialects (e.g. /ɛ/, /æ/, /u/, /aj/) • better control of stimulus duration

  25. Future directions • Replicate with… • all low frequency words, which are likely to be imitated (Goldinger 1998) • dialects that are less mutually familiar e.g. North, Midland and South • focus on vowels that differentiate dialects (e.g. /ɛ/, /æ/, /u/, /aj/) • better control of stimulus duration

  26. References • Babel, M. E. (2009). Phonetic and social selectivity in speech accommodation. Unpublished dissertation, University of California, Berkeley. • Baker, R. E., & Bradlow, A. R. (2009). Variability in word duration as a function of probability, speech style, and prosody. Language and Speech, 52(4), 391-413. • Bell, A. (1984). Language style as audience design. Language in Society, 13, 145-204. • Clopper, C. G., & Bradlow, A. R. (2008). Perception of dialect variation in noise: Intelligibility and classification. Language and Speech, 51(3), 175-198. • Clopper, C. G., Carter, A. K., Dillon, C. M., Hernandez, L. R., Pisoni, D. B., Clarke, C. M., et al. (2002). The Indiana Speech Project: An overview of the development of a multi-talker multi-dialect speech corpus. In D. B. Pisoni (Ed.), Speech Research Laboratory Progress Report 25: Research on Spoken Language Processing (pp. 367-380). Bloomington, IN: Speech Research Laboratory, Indiana University. • Floccia, C., Goslin, J., Girard, F., & Konopczynski, G. (2006). Does a regional accent perturb speech processing? Journal of Experimenal Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(5), 1276-1293. • Fowler, C. A., & Housum, J. (1987). Talkers' signaling of "new" and "old" words in speech and listeners' perception and use of the distinction. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 489-504. • Giles, H. (1973). Accent mobility: a model and some data. Anthropological Linguistics, 15, 87-105. • Giles, H., Coupland, N., & Coupland, J. (1991). Accommodation theory: Communication, context, and consequence. In H. Giles, J. Coupland & N. Coupland (Eds.), Contexts of Accommodation (pp. 1-68). • Goldinger, S. D. (1998). Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access. Psychological Review, 105(2), 251-279. • Goldinger, S. D. (2000). The role of perceptual episodes in lexical processing. Paper presented at the Proceedings of SWAP (Spoken Word Access Processes). • Jungers, M. K., Palmer, C., & Speer, S. R. (2002). Time after time: The coordinating influence of tempo in music and speech. Cognitive Processing, 2, 21-35. • Nielsen, K. Y. (2011). Specificity and abstractness of VOT imitation. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 132-142. • Nye, P. W., & Fowler, C. A. (2003). Shadowing latency and imitation: the effect of familiarity with the phonetic patterning of English. Journal of Phonetics, 31, 63-79. • Pardo, J. S. (2006). On phonetic convergence during conversational interaction. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 2382-2393. • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2004). The interactive-alignment model: Developments and refinements. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(2), 212-219. • Pierrehumbert, J. B. (2002). Word-specific phonetics. In C. Gussenhoven & N. Warner (Eds.), Papers in Laboratory Phonology VII (pp. 101-139). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. • Sumner, M., & Samuel, A. G. (2009). The effect of experience on the perception and representation of dialect variants. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 487-501. • Tilsen, S. (2009). Subphonemic and cross-phonemic priming in vowel shadowing: Evidence for the involvement of exemplars in production. Journal of Phonetics, 37, 276-296.

  27. plots of acoustic correlations

  28. plots of acoustic correlations

  29. plots of acoustic correlations

  30. Background: Dialect Processing Processing benefit for familiar dialects: • Unfamiliar regional dialects can create temporary RT lag in a lexical decision task (Floccia et al., 2006). • Unfamiliar dialects are less intelligible in noise than familiar dialects (Clopper & Bradlow, 2008).

  31. Background: Dialect Processing • Mere exposure to a dialect can improve performance on lexical decision tasks. • R-ful speakers in NY are faster and more accurate in lexical decision using R-less stimuli if they have exposure to R-less speech (Sumner & Samuel, 2009). • “Familiar” = both perception and production

  32. Statistics • Separate linear mixed effects models predicting imitation score for each acoustic measure. • Fixed Effects: • shadower dialect region • target dialect region • target measurement (ex. duration of target) • gender • Random Effects: • item (word) • subject

  33. Vowel centralization • Hard to distinguish between imitation and centralization. • Second mention reduction? (Fowler & Housum 1987; Baker & Bradlow 2009) • only of vowel quality, not duration • shadowed productions were not shorter than the baseline

  34. AXB task: measured imitation • What did the AXB shadowers imitate? • vowel quality n.s. • vowel duration n.s. Mobile shad: stim duration t = 4.372 • onset duration t = 3.305 • coda duration t = 5.112 • midpoint f0 n.s. • f0 trajectory n.s.

More Related