1 / 11

Critique of May 10, 2000 Report on the Yale Hemorrhagic Stroke Project (HSP)

Critique of May 10, 2000 Report on the Yale Hemorrhagic Stroke Project (HSP). Brian L. Strom, M.D., M.P.H. Chair and Professor, Department of Biostatistics and Epidemiology Director, Center for Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics

esma
Download Presentation

Critique of May 10, 2000 Report on the Yale Hemorrhagic Stroke Project (HSP)

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Critique of May 10, 2000 Report on theYale Hemorrhagic Stroke Project (HSP) Brian L. Strom, M.D., M.P.H. Chair and Professor, Department of Biostatistics and Epidemiology Director, Center for Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics Professor of Biostatistics and Epidemiology, Professor of Medicine Professor of Pharmacology University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine Consultant, Whitehall-Robins Healthcare

  2. Critique of May 10, 2000 Report on theYale Hemorrhagic Stroke Project (HSP) • Initiated due to spontaneous reports • Appropriate, because of severe limitations of spontaneous reports in evaluating cause • Huge, ambitious study • Thoughtfully designed • Methodologically problematic • Chance • Confounding • Bias

  3. Chance • Marginal power: designed to detect OR=5.0 with a 1-tailed test • Very small numbers of exposed cases = fragile results • 3 (or 5) co-equal aims  alpha should be 0.0166 (0.01) • Inconsistent results in subgroups by gender and indication, suggest chance as an explanation • “Dose-response relationship” never tested statistically

  4. Key Results-All PPA CVA No CVA 27 33 Exposed 664 1310 Not exposed Adj OR = 1.49 (p>0.05)

  5. Key Results-By Indication Appetite Suppressants Cough/Cold CVA No CVA CVA No CVA 22 32 Exposed 6 1 664 1310 664 1310 Not exposed Adj OR = 1.23 (p>0.05) Adj OR = 15.92 (p=0.013)

  6. Key Results-Among Women Appetite Suppressants All PPA: First Use CVA No CVA CVA No CVA 7 4 6 1 Exposed 355 713 355 713 Not exposed Adj OR = 3.13 (p=0.042) Adj OR = 16.58 (p=0.011)

  7. Confounding • Controlled using conditional logistic regression, but sample size too small for valid modeling • Better approach would have used stratification and/or exclusion

  8. Misclassification Bias • ?valid to combine subarachnoid hemorrhage and primary intracerebral hemorrhage?

  9. Information Bias • Valid drug histories always difficult to collect retrospectively • Valid drug histories would be much harder to collect from stroke patients, resulting in unequal recall • Great effort taken to collect good data, but validation procedure assures specificity, not sensitivity, and very few missed exposures in controls would eliminate association

  10. Selection Bias • Properly done case-control studies should be population-based • Cases • not representative of the entire population, unlike controls: ?increased exposures • ?completeness of identified • Only 41% of those identified, were enrolled • Controls: • no info given on process and success of RDD

  11. Conclusions • Ambitious and well described • Major risk of information bias and selection bias • Underpowered  fragile, subject to change in results with even small errors • At best, this study suggests the possibility of an association between use of this common drug and this very uncommon outcome. It does not prove it. • This association remains uncertain.

More Related