200 likes | 321 Views
Improving Social Skills by Building Fluency on Deictic Relational Classes. Donny Newsome, MA University of Nevada, Reno. The Challenge of Teaching Social Skills. Slippery – difficult to define Subtle Contextually dependent Subjective Impacts Quality of Life Collateral problem behaviors
E N D
Improving Social Skills by Building Fluency on Deictic Relational Classes Donny Newsome, MA University of Nevada, Reno
The Challenge of Teaching Social Skills • Slippery – difficult to define • Subtle • Contextually dependent • Subjective • Impacts Quality of Life • Collateral problem behaviors • Verbal abuse • Theft • Property destruction
Traditional Approaches • Component Skills Deficit Model - Views knowledge of rules as being key component skills of the broader social repertoire • “eye contact is good, but not for too long” • “don’t stare” • “do unto others….” • “always say please and thank you”
The Problems with Rule-Based Approaches • Infinite number of rules • Limited applicability of a single rule • ‘always say please and thank you…..well, not always….just most of the time….well, really just when it is socially appropriate to do so…but not at times when it isn’t…..’ • Rigidity – Lack of contextual sensitivity • Insensitivity to changes in contingencies not described in the rule • (Haas & Hayes, 2006; Hayes, Brownstein, Haas, & Greenway, 1986; Hayes, Strosal & Wilson, 1999; Skinner, 1957)
Alternative: Experiential Contact • Non-specific feedback on performance, but not rules • (Azrin & Hayes, 1984; Rosenfarb, Hayes & Linehan, 1989) • Outperformed rule-based strategies *Requires a certain minimal repertoire to be sensitive to feedback and subtle differences in social contingencies
New Conceptualization of Component Skills Deficit Model • Emerging approaches: Component skills identified at a more fundamental level of cognitive processes • Similar to Johnson & Layng (1992) definition of tool skills: “the most basic elements of more complex skills” (pg 1479).
New Conceptualization of Skill Deficit Model • Deficits are at the level of basic verbal processes (relational responding), not in knowledge of rules • Basic relational operants are not situation-specific • Allows for a generative approach to social skill acquisition • Promotes meaningful contact and sensitivity to subtle social cues and contingencies • Making room for shaping to occur
RFT – Perspective Taking • Deictic Frames • 3 Types of relations I – you Here – there Now – then • 3 Levels of Complexity Simple Reversed Double-reversed
RFT – Perspective Taking • Validity in Evidence: • Performance on ToM tasks in social anhedonia and schizophrenia (Barnes-Holmes, et al. 2004; Villatte, et al. 2008; Villatte, et al. 2010; Weil, et al. 2010) • Deficits in perspective-taking tasks in ASD relative to controls (Rehfeldt, et al 2007) • IQ (RFT–PT) (Gore, et al. 2010)
Case Study - Background • 24 yr old Male, JP • Autism, Mild MR, ADHD, Speech impediment (stutter) • Problem Behaviors: • Verbal abuse • Stealing • Property destruction • Refusals • Acquisition Targets: • Appropriate conversation skills • Coping skills • Compromising
Case Study – Initial Protocol • Began with standard differential reinforcement protocol combined with replacement behavior training (RBT) • RBT protocols included role-playing with feedback and hypothetical-situation exercises • Some acquisition targets moved, but problem behaviors also increased
Case Study – Revised Protocol • Included fluency training on simple deictic relations • Daily training on I – You relations • Weekly probes for Here – There and Now – Then relations • Additional fluency programs for socially relevant skills • F/S Emotion terms • H/S Complete sentence with emotive term • H/S Emotion for event • F/S Positive statements • H/S What you can do to help • F/S Thoughts about standing in line • F/S Thoughts about life in 10 years
Differential Reinforcement + RBT + Deictic Baseline Differential Reinforcement + RBT Replacement Behaviors Problem Behaviors
Replacement Bx Differential Reinforcement + RBT + Deictic Baseline Differential Reinforcement + RBT Problem Bx Differential Reinforcement + RBT + Deictic Differential Reinforcement + RBT Baseline
Case Study - Results • Targeting deictic relational skills appeared to improve sensitivity to programmed social contingencies • This was accomplished by only training simple relations • Also found that training all 3 deictic relations was not necessary
I - You Here – There & Now - Then Incorrect Responses
Case Study – Caveats & Questions • Idiosyncratic? • ‘True’ fluency was difficult to measure due to stuttering issue • Unable to say which programs were critical to success • Incremental utility of training reversed and double-reversed relations
Case Study - Contributions • Practical Utility • Value of a fluency-based approach and SCC measurement system • Utility of time-series analysis
References • Azrin, R.D., Hayes, S.C. (1984). The Discrimination of Interest Within a Heterosexual Interaction: Training, Generalization, and Effects of Social Skills. Behavior Therapy, 15, 173-184. • Gore, J.N., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Murphy, G. (2010). The relationship between intellectual functioning and relational perspective-taking. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 10-1, 1-17. • Barnes-Holmes Y., McHugh, L., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004). Perspective-taking and theory of mind: A relational frame account. The Behavior Analyst Today, 5, 15-25. • Haas, J. R., Hayes, S. C. (2006). When knowing you are doing well hinders performance: Exploring the interaction between rules and feedback. Journal of Organizational Behavior Management, 26 (1,2), pp. 91-111. • Hayes, S.C., Brownstein, A.J., Haas, J.R. & Greenway, D.E. (1986). Instructions, multiple schedules, and extinction: Distinguishing rule-governed from schedule- controlled behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46(2): 137-147. • Hayes, S.C, Strosal, K.D., Wilson, K.G. (1999). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy. The Guilford Press, New York, NY. • Johnson, K.R., Layng, T.V. (1992). Breaking the structuralist barrier, literacy and numeracy with fluency. American Psychologist, 47(11), 1475-1490.
References • McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y. & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004). Perspective-taking as relational responding: A developmental profile. The Psychological Record, 54, 115-144. • Rehfeldt, R.A., Dillen, J.E., Ziomek, M.M. & Kowalchuk, R.K. (2007). Assessing relational learning deficits in perspective-taking with high functioning autism spectrum disorder. The Psychological Record, 57, 23-47. • Rosenfarb, I.S., Hayes, S.C., Linehan, M.M. (1989). Instructions and experiential feedback in the treatment of social skills deficits in adults. Psychotherapy, 26(2), 242-251. • Skinner, B.F. (1957). Verbal Behavior. Copley Publishing Group. Acton, Massachusetts. • Villatte, M., Monestes, J., McHuch, L., Baque, E.F., Loas, G. (2008). Assessing deictic relational responding in social anhedonia: A functional approach to the development of theory of mind impairments. International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 4-4, 360-373. • Villatte, M., Monestes, J., McHuch, L., Baque, E.F., Loas, G. (2010). Adopting the perspective of another in belief attribution: The contribution of relational frame theory to the understanding of impairments in schizophrenia. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41, 125-134. • Weil, T. M. & Hayes, S. C. (Under Review) Impact of training deictic frames on Theory of Mind in Children. Psychological Record.