660 likes | 864 Views
Mathematics and the human brain Problem posing and problem solving in mathematics. Student concerns: 27 students Students per group. 9 groups 9 15 mins = 135 mins = 2 hours 15 mins ≅ 2 ¼ hours Time remaining after today’s session = 11 3 hours = 33 hours
E N D
Mathematics and the human brainProblem posing and problem solving in mathematics
Student concerns: • 27 students • Students per group. 9 groups • 9 15 mins = 135 mins • = 2 hours 15 mins ≅ 2 ¼ hours • Time remaining after today’s session = 11 3 hours = 33 hours • Subtract 3 hours for last session for practical work. • 30 hours remain.
Student concerns: Time remaining for lectures = 24 7 = 17 hours Number of topics remaining = 13 topics Number of topics per session = (13 topics 17 hours) 3 hours = 2.3 ≅ 3 topics (round up for emergencies)
Please go to my website http://sites.google.com/site/ctmathelp . The notes been loaded on the teachers page. B.Ednotes.Some have difficulty accessing the links fromthe power point file.If you are using the power point programme and you clickon a website , you may need to come out of power pointand then click on internet browser at the botom of the screen. The word files are done in word 2007. I have saved in word 2003 to allow access to those who do not have word 2007. You also need adobe 9.
Assessment/ Lesson plans/ 15 minute teaching Practical activities e.g. 15 minute session simulating with B.Ed class group teaching infants or junior school pupils a concept or reinforcement lesson while developing higher order skills.
A portfolio of 5 uniquely prepared and presented / shared mathematics lessons. Each must be accompanied by • a rationale for the method tried, • a description of the students involved, • their learning styles, • age, class level, who they are; • include your reflections on the learning process, • assessment and evaluation.
E-MAILS RECEIVED FROM Lisa Llanos Michelle De Gourville Ferida Welch Olivia Guillen Corann Browne Germaine Emmanuel-Letren Julia Andrews Patricia Prescott Jamie Peters
E-MAILS RECEIVED FROM Sr. Olivia Oculien Natasha Harding Shellyann Gill Patricia Belcon Phillis Griffith Coromoto Fernandez – Sirju Francisca Monsegue Hazel Warner-Paul Patricia Joseph-Charles
GROUP 1 • Olivia Guillen • CharmaineFigeroux • Shelly Gill • Lisa Llanos • GROUP 2 • Corann Browne • Nathalie Faria • Natasha Harding • GROUP 3 • Julia AndrewsPatricia Joseph Charles Phyllis Griffith
GROUP 4 • Patricia PrescottSr. Olivia Marie OculienMichelle De Gourville • GROUP 5 • Coromoto Fernandez - Sirju • Francisca MonsegueJamie Peters
E-mail contacts Review of last lecture : language, concepts, procedures, questions.
Set Induction: • Mathematics and the human brain • Evidence accumulated more recently suggests that the effects of sexhormones on brain organization occur so early in life, that from the start, the environment is acting on differently wired brains in boys and girls.
Such effects make evaluating the role of experience, independent of physiological predisposition, a difficult if not dubious task. • The biological bases of sex differences in brain and behaviour have become much better known through increasing numbers of behavioural, neurological and endocrinological studies.
We know, for instance, from observations of both humans and nonhumans that • males are generally more aggressive than females, • young males engage in more rough-and-tumble play than females • females are more nurturing • in general males are better at a variety of spatial or navigational tasks.
Mathematics and the human brain Tall, D. ( 2004). THINKING THROUGH THREE WORLDS OF MATHEMATICS http://www.emis.ams.org/proceedings/PME28/RR/RR213_Tall.pdf http://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=dWXPJvhco_UC&oi=fnd&pg=PA133&dq=Mathematics+and+the+human+brain&ots=neyWMnCWVh&sig=mKKNmE0veTswxqeUkt3mZfT2S54#v=onepage&q=Mathematics%20and%20the%20human%20brain&f=false
Theorists, such as Piaget (1965), Dienes (1960) and Bruner (1966), have something to say that had particular relevance in mathematics. At one time, Piagetian theories held sway, with an emphasis on successive stages of development and a particular focus on the transitions between stages.
Piagetian theory was a tripartite theory of abstraction: • empirical abstraction focusing on how the child constructs meaning for the properties of objects, • pseudo-empiricalabstraction, focusing on construction of meaning for the properties of actions on objects, and • reflective abstraction focused on the idea of how ‘actions and operations become thematized objects of thought or assimilation’ (Piaget, 1985, p. 49).
Meanwhile, Bruner focused on three distinct ways in which ‘the individual translates experience into a model of the world’, namely, enactive, iconic and symbolic (Bruner 1966, p.10). The foundational symbolic systemis language, with two important symbolic systems especially relevant to mathematics:number and logic (ibid. pp. 18, 19).
In his research on the development of children (1966), Bruner proposed three modes of representation: enactive representation (action-based), iconic representation (image-based), symbolic representation (language- based). Rather than neatly delineated stages, the modes of representation are integrated and only loosely sequential as they "translate" into each other.
Symbolic representation remains the ultimate mode, for it "is clearly the most mysterious of the three." Bruner's theory suggests it is efficacious when faced with new material to follow a progression from enactive to iconic to symbolic representation; this holds true even for adult learners.
A true instructional designer, Bruner's work also suggests that a learner (even of a very young age) is capable of learning any material so long as the instruction is organized appropriately, in sharp contrast to the beliefs of Piaget and other stage theorists. Like Bloom's Taxonomy, Bruner suggests a system of coding in which people form a hierarchical arrangement of related categories. Each successively higher level of categories becomes more specific, echoing Benjamin Bloom's understanding of knowledge acquisition as well as the related idea of instructional scaffolding.
In accordance with this understanding of learning, Bruner proposed the spiral curriculum, a teaching approach in which each subject or skill area is revisited at intervals, at a more sophisticated level each time.
EfraimFischbein, was from the very beginning interested in three distinct aspects ofmathematical thinking: fundamental intuitions that he saw as being widely shared, The algorithmsthat give us power in computation and symbolic manipulation, and The formalaspect of axioms, definitions and formal proof (Fischbein, 1987).
Richard Skemp, balanced his professional knowledge of mathematics and psychology with both theory and practice, also developed a general theory of increasingly sophisticated human learning (Skemp, 1971, 1979). He saw the individual having receptors to receive information from the environment and effectors to act on the environment forming a system he referred to as ‘delta-one’; a higher level system of mental receptors and effectors (delta-two) reflected on the operations of delta-one.
This two level system incorporates three distinct types of activity: • perception (input), • action (output) and • reflection, • which itself involves higher levels of perception and action.
The emphases in these three-way interpretations of cognitive growth are very different, but there are underlying resonances that appear throughout. First there is a concern about how human beings come to construct and make sense of mathematicalideas.
Then there are different ways in which this construction develops, from real worldperception and action, real-world enactive and iconic representations, fundamental intuitions that seem to be shared,
via the developing sophistication of language to support more abstract concepts including the symbolism of number (and later developments),
the increasing sophistication of description, definition and deduction that culminates in formal axiomatic theories.
In geometry, van Hiele (1959, 1986) has traced cognitive development through increasingly sophisticated succession of levels. His theory begins with young children perceiving objects as whole gestalts, noticing various properties that can be described and subsequently used in verbal definitions to give hierarchies of figures, with verbal deductions that designate how, if certain properties hold, then others follow, culminating in more rigorous, formal axiomatic mathematics.
Meanwhile, process-object theories such as Dubinsky’s APOS theory (Czarnochaet al., 1999) and the operational-structural theory of Sfard (1991) gave new impetus inthe construction of mathematical objects from thematized processes in the manner ofPiaget’s reflective abstraction.
Gray & Tall (1994) brought a new emphasis on therole of symbols, particularly in arithmetic and algebra, that act as a pivot between a • do-able process and a think-able concept that is manipulable as a mental object (a procept).
Two further strands also emerged, one encouraged by the American Congress declaring 1990-2000 as ‘the decade of the brain’ in which resources were offered to expand research into brain activity.
The other related to a focus on embodiment in cognitive science where the linguist Lakoff worked with colleagues to declare that allthinking processes are embodied in biological activity.
Brain imaging techniques were used to determine low grain maps of where brain activities are occurring. Such studies focused mainly on elementary arithmetic activities (egDehaene 1997, Butterworth 1999).
Other studies revealed how logical thinking, particularly when the negation of logical statements is involved, causes a shift in brain activity from the visual sensory areas at the back of the brain to the more generalised frontal cortex (Houdéet al, 2000).
This reveals a distinct change in brain activity, consistent with a significant shift from sensory information to formal thinking.
At the other end of the scale, studies of young babies (Wynn,1992) revealed a built-in sense of numerosity for distinguishing small configurations of ‘twoness’ and ‘threeness’, long before the child had any language.
The human brain has visual areas that perceive different colours, shades, changes in shade, edges,outlines and objects, which can be followed dynamically as they move. • Implicit in this structure is the ability to recognize small groups of objects (one, two or three), providing the young child with a fundamental intuition for small numbers.
In the second development, Lakoff and colleagues theorized that human embodiment suffused/ inundated all human thinking, culminating in an analysis of Where MathematicsComes From (Lakoff & Nunez, 2000).
Suddenly all mathematics is claimed to be embodied. This is a powerful idea on the one hand, but a classification with only one category is not helpful in making distinctions. • If one takes ‘embodiment’ in its everyday meaning, then it relates more to the use of physical senses and actions and to visuo-spatial ideas in Bruner’s two categories of enactive and iconic representations.
Following through van Hiele’s development, the visual embodiment of physical objects becomes more sophisticated and concepts such as ‘straight line’ take on a conceptual meaning of being perfectly straight, and having no thickness, in a way that cannot occur in the real world.
JOURNEYS THROUGH THE THREE WORLDS JOURNEYS THROUGH THE THREE WORLDS.docx
Process and Product Process and Product.docx
Ministry of education SEA guidelines [mathematics] • http://www.moe.gov.tt/parent_guides/SEA-GUIDELINES.pdf • Van de Walle, J.A. (2004). Elementary and Middle School Mathematics: Teaching • Developmentally. Boston: Pearson Education Inc. • Of doing mathematics pp. 13-14.
Problem posing and problem solving in mathematics http://jwilson.coe.uga.edu/emt725/PSsyn/PSsyn.html http://www.emis.de/proceedings/PME28/RR/RR117_Lin.pdf
The reformed curriculum suggested that every instructional activity is an assessment opportunity for teachers and a learning opportunity for students (NCTM, 2000). The movement emphasized classroom assessment in gathering information on which teachers can inform their further instruction (NCTM, 1995).
Assessment integral to instruction contributes significantly to all students’ mathematics learning. The new vision of assessment suggested that knowing how these assessment processes take place should become a focus of teacher education programs.
Problem-posing involves generating new problems and reformatting a given problems (Silver, 1994). The quality of problems in which students generated depends on the given tasks (Leung & Silver, 1997).