1 / 23

The educational gender gap, catch up and labour market performance

The educational gender gap, catch up and labour market performance. Martyn Andrews (University of Manchester) , Steve Bradley, Dave Stott & Jim Taylor (Lancaster University). The educational gender gap. Issues Performance of girls is superior to boys and getting wider

hedia
Download Presentation

The educational gender gap, catch up and labour market performance

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. The educational gender gap, catch up and labour market performance Martyn Andrews(University of Manchester), Steve Bradley, Dave Stott & Jim Taylor(Lancaster University)

  2. The educational gender gap • Issues • Performance of girls is superior to boys and getting wider • Concern about low achieving boys • Girls do better in ‘language’ based subjects, boys do better in Maths & Science • Even if girls outperform boys, does it matter if they are discriminated against in the labour market?

  3. The educational gender gap • Objectives • Use biannual YCS (1985-2001) & NPD (2002-03) • 1. Define & measure the gender gap and document how it changes through time • 2. Explain how the gap changes when we control for • Observable effects – individual, family, school, neighbourhood • Unobservable effects • School-level (e.g. discipline, tiering, streaming • Individual-level (e.g. attitudes, motivation) • 3. Repeat 1 & 2 for subject groups • 4. Measure & explain how the gap changes during the educational process • Age 11-16 (at KS2, KS3, KS4)

  4. Previous research • Educational • Descriptive studies e.g. Gorard et al (1999) • School effectiveness e.g. Wong et al (2002) • Qualitative / case studies e.g. OFSTED (2003) • Organisation, teaching & learning, curriculum & assessment • School organisation • Culture of laddishness • Idiosyncratic school effects • Home background • Economics e.g. Dolton et al (1999), Burgess et al (2004)

  5. Data & methodology • Estimate education production functions • Outcome = function of: • Girl (gap) • Individual characteristics • School characteristics • Neighbourhood characteristics • Unobserved individual-level effects • Unobserved school-level effects • Are there correlations between girl and (observable & unobservable) effects? • Zero – gap is the published figure • Girl & personal (zero?) • Girl & school (sorting?) • Girl & unobserved individual effects (motivation) • Girl & unobserved school effects (sorting?) • Unobserved individual-level & unobserved school-level effects

  6. Data • Pooled cross-section (YCS) data (1985-2001) • YCS2-3 – GCE/CSE • YCS4+ -- GCSE • Observed variables • Individual – gender, ethnicity, age • Family – parental occupation, single parent, housing tenure • School – Pupil-teacher ratio, pupil composition, size, competition • Neighbourhood – unemployment rate, occupational mix • YCS6-11 observe the same school up to 6 times – school level unobservables

  7. Data • NPD 2002 & 2003 • Observe KS2, KS3 & GCSE results • Population • Advantages: • Control for (estimate?) unobserved individual effects • 41,000 pupils move schools • Identify individual & school level unobservables • But … few individual-level covariates

  8. Outcomes – measures of educational performance • Pass/fail for each subject (grade C +) • Number A*-C GCSEs – all subjects • 5 + A*-C GCSEs – headline figure • Points score – distribution (A*=7, etc.)

  9. Absolute versus relative gaps • Debate • Educationalists label the absolute gap as the ‘politicians error’ • Absolute gap increases as relative gap falls • Absolute gap is correct • Note the increase in the gap from the introduction of GCSE

  10. Econometric findings - observables

  11. What explains the gender gap (differential)? • Selective schools have a very large effect on attainment • Single sex schools have a large, but smaller, effect • Neither of these effects contribute much to the gender gap • Other observable differences between girls and boys (e.g. family background, poverty) do not explain the gap • Are the findings genuine? Biased sample for YCS but we observe similar effects for NPD (population)

  12. The story so far • Observable differences between girls & boys do not explain the gap • Girls must therefore behave differently prior to GCSEs • 1. Choice of secondary school • 2. Subject level gaps at GCSE • 3. Differences in exam performance between KS2 & KS4

  13. 1. Choice of school • Control for school-level unobservables • YCS6-11 & NPD1-2 (panels) • Controlling for school level unobservables is important • level not trend • Discipline, tiering, streaming • Between 1991-2001 the gender gap is halved • E.g. YCS10 = 0.04 versus 0.10 • Implication: Has the quasi-market (ERA, 1988) meant that girls are marginally more attractive to better schools? • Un-testable because of lack of linked school data prior to 1991

  14. 2. Subject level gaps at GCSE

  15. 2. Subject level gaps at GCSE • Data shows that girls outperform boys in languages, English & vocational subjects • ‘One-off’ GCE-GCSE effect disadvantaging boys – languages, science, maths • Since 1988 the gap has increased at the same rate – girls catch-up in maths & science • Controlling for observable & unobservable differences lowers the gap by one-tenth of a GCSE grade • Girls ahead in English, languages & vocational, level in humanities & behind in Maths and Science

  16. 3. Differences in exam performance between KS2 & KS4 • Maths, English, Science at KS2, KS3 & KS4 (population) • See Table on KS2-4 • Gaps at GCSE: English (0.63), Maths (0.03) and Science (0.06) • At KS2: Girls better in English (0.23), behind in Maths (-0.07) & Science (-0.04) • Girls improve between KS3 & KS4 in all subjects, but only in English between KS2 & KS3

  17. Differences in exam performance • Controlling for school & pupil-level unobservables • 1. Correlation between ‘Girl’ & individual-level = 0! • But, disaggregating we find that girls are unobservably better in English and worse in Maths & Science • Note that KS2 & KS3 do not test other ‘girl-good’ subjects – see YCS results • 2. The correlation between unobserved-school level & unobserved individual-level effects is greater than zero • Unobservably good pupils go to unobservable good schools (i.e. middle class parents, catchment areas) • 3. The correlation between ‘Girl’ & unobserved school-level effects is greater than zero (see YCS results) • Girls go to unobservably better schools • Girls are observably better at KS2 – schools therefore select them

  18. Conclusions & implications for policy • 1. Gender gap emerges once the GCSE system is introduced • Learning & assessment methods favour girls • 2. Girls are better than boys • A) English • B) Selected into unobservably better schools • 3. No effect of single sex schooling • 4. Selective schools & poverty have a small effect on the gap • 5. Gap is greatest in English & languages and has closed in Maths & Science • 6. Unobserved differences between schools (e.g. discipline, tiering, streaming) are important – YCS only

  19. Speculation • A) Introduction of GCSE system created the gap • B) Quasi-market exacerbated the gap • changed incentives facing schools • select the best – girls • Cumulative & self-perpetuating • Girls go to good schools • But the gap stabilises • Shocks A & B eventually burn out (equilibrium) • The introduction of KS2 helps boys (fewer ‘girl-good’ tests), which means they also sort into ‘good’ schools

More Related