650 likes | 760 Views
Space. Lecture 4. Introduction. One of our earliest and most basic achievements as infants is to acquire an understanding of objects and of the way in which they relate to each other in physical space.
E N D
Space Lecture 4
Introduction • One of our earliest and most basic achievements as infants is to acquire an understanding of objects and of the way in which they relate to each other in physical space. • The kind of concepts represented by words such as up, down, in, out, on, off and so on are the building blocks on which we construct our mental models of the physical world.
Introduction • Piaget (1936) recognized the fundamental importance of these concepts when he characterized the first stage of cognitive development as “sensorimotor knowledge”. • We use space as a domain for structuring other less concrete aspects of our experience.
Introduction • For example, when we say that someone occupies a ‘high’ position in society, we are using the up-down axis as a means of talking about social status. If someone says that they are ‘in trouble’, they are treating ‘trouble’ as a container and themselves as a contained object. • If we say that we have a ‘close’ relationship with someone, we are constructing the notion of intimacy in terms of physical proximity.
In • To refer to a situation where one object (trajector射体) is contained within another (the landmark界标). • In is also used in a whole range of situations where there is only an approximation to this ideal meaning.
In • The cat in the house • The bird in the garden • The flowers in the vase • The chair in the corner • The water in the vase • The crack in the vase • The finger in the ring
In • In the last example, it is odd to conceptualise the situation in terms of the finger being ‘in’ the ring. • The finger functions as a fixed entity (LM) and the ring is placed as TR. • These examples show that, in order to explain the forms that we use to code these situations in language, we need to go beyond the level of surface topographical relationships.
In • Background knowledge involving the relevant functional relationships is crucial to an understanding of the forms of coding, which reflect subtle aspects of everyday human experience. • The bulb in the socket • The jar in the lid
In • The topographical relationships are similar in each case, but only the second example is the relationship constructed in terms of containment. • Our background knowledge is crucial to an understanding of the relevant linguistic patterns.
On • The pen on the desk • The writing on the paper • The poster on the wall • The wrinkle on the face • The fly on the ceiling • The writing is applied to the paper in a manner that bears some similarity to the way in which a pen is placed on a desk.
On • The wall, like the papers, forms a background, with the writing and the poster as foregrounded or displayed entities. • The last example is unusual in that the topological relationship between the ceiling and the fly is precisely the opposite of that which holds in the normal situation coded by on.
At • The function of at is to locate two entities at precisely the same point in space and construe them as geometric points. • John is at the supermarket. • John is in the supermarket. • As we move away from objects in our visual field, their image on the retina grows smaller, so that at a given distance they begin to approximate to a point.
At • Let’s meet at the library. • This distinction is lost when the building is construed as a point. • The café is at the highway. • *The café is on the highway. • The café is located at the place where the path intersects with the highway at some point ahead, a location that is quite naturally conceptualized as a point.
At • The concept of path is also present in the following examples, but in a more abstract form. • The bird has a white band at its neck. • The bird is at the top of the tree. • There are bubbles at the surface. • The conceptualizer scans the body of the bird and comes across a white band when this scanning process reaches the neck. • There is implicit movement through the liquid, encountering bubbles when it reaches the surface.
Construal of objects and spatial relationships • The choice of one preposition over another is a matter not of the objective properties of the observed situation but of the way in which the various elements of the situation and the relationships between them are constructed. • (1) the words on the page • (2) the words in the margin • The paper is construed as a background serving to display the (foregrounded) words makes the concept of support more salient than that of containment.
Construal of objects and spatial relationships • The function of a margin is not to display text but to define a particular area of the page, so that in cases such as (2) it it the notion of containment that is salient. • (3) John is in the bus/train/plane/boat. • (4) John is on the bus/train/plane/boat.
Construal of objects and spatial relationships • Vehicles can be construed either as container or as supporting surfaces. • (3) and (4) involve salience, framing and construal. The use of in is favoured if the vehicle is stationary. • When on is used, it is because the role of the vehicle is more salient than its role as a container.
Construal of objects and spatial relationships • (5) He hit the ball through the outfield. • (6) He rolled the ball across the pitch. • (7) The pushed the mower over the lawn. • Through involves construal of LM as a container, whereas across and over are construed as a supporting surface.
Construal of objects and spatial relationships • Different languages often make different choices. Convention does indeed play a significant role. The notion of construal itself predicts that there should be some degree of conventionality at work, since in situation that lend themselves to different construals, it is to be expected that different languages will make different choices for their preferred coding.
Introduction • We shall look at the ways in which we use spatial terms and spatial concepts to structure non-structural meanings. • 1a. The sun is out; The stars are out. • 1b. The light is out; The fire is out; He blew the candles out. • 2a. He threw out a suggestion to the meeting. • 2b. They threw out Bill’s suggestion.
Introduction • It seems odd that the sun or stars are ‘out’ they are visible, but when lights, candles, or fires are ‘out’, they are invisible. • 3. He blew up the balloon. • 4. He rolled up the carpet. • Up in 3 is part of an expression that refers to an object getting bigger, whereas in 4 the object gets smaller.
Introduction • Prepositions that normally express opposite meanings sometimes express similar meanings: • She filled in the form. • She filled out the form. • Sometimes prepositions express meanings that seem to be quite unrelated to each other.
Introduction • The students dropped in. • The students dropped out. • Prepositional usage has been claimed to be simply chaotic in English. A careful analysis shows that the situation is less unruly than it appears at first sight.
Out • The core meaning of out involves an image schema that an entity (TR) is located outside a container (LM). The out relationship is sometimes static and sometimes dynamic. • The cat is out of the house. • The cat went out of the house.
Out • One form of extension from the dynamic situation involves situations in which an entity expands in size or comes to occupy a greater area. The TR is reflexive. • The lava spread out. • Roll out the carpet.
Out • Another semantic extension involves a process whereby an entity moves away from a landmark without any obvious notion of exit. • She set out for Shanghai. • He reached out to grab it. • “The sun is out” involves applications of the basic notion of exit from location.
Out • LM is a container that is inaccessible to an observer and TR is an entity that is deemed to have emerged from this area into the observer’s perceptual field. • “The fire is out” involves movement of an entity away from the observer into the hidden area.
Out • From unknown to known • I worked out a solution to the problem. • Becoming unavailable • Our supplies have run out. • The notion of Exit • 1) Exit from perceptual field; 2) from cognitive field; 3) from area of control or availability; 4) from normal state.
Fill in and fill out the form • A process whereby material is inserted into spaces in the form • A process that causes the form to increase in size as material is added. • Speak up and speak out
through • The image schema for through involves the path of an entity (TR) in physical space as it enters a container and then moves from the point of entry to a point at the opposite side of the container. • The train (TR) rushed through the tunnel (LM). • The postman pushed the letter through the letterbox.
through • John was looking through the window. (a virtual trajectory) • John was walking through the grass. (a notional line) • One factor in the growth of semantic network is the activation of elements in the knowledge base associated with core meaning.
through • Another component of the knowledge frame is the idea that TR traverses LM only with difficulty. • We headached our way through peak-hour traffic. • John sold the house to Mary through Smith’s. (landmark as instrument) • A small child could not sit through a four-hour dinner. (landmark as ordeal)
Radial Categories • A property of such networks associated with out, up and through is that they are structured around a core meaning (prototype). • The property of radiality is characteristic of many types of linguistic and conceptual categories. The prototype model recognizes that category membership is a gradient phenomenon, such that some members of a category are more central members than others.
-able • Likeable, washable, readable, solvable (able to be verbed) • Payable (due to be paid) • comparable: a part of the situation as a whole has been semantically foregrounded at the expenses of those aspects of the situation that relate to the process itself.
-able • The meaning of the combined form is the outcome of an interaction between the frames associated with the two forms. • The background knowledge associated with the verb modulates the meaning of the suffix in a particular direction peculiar to that verb. • Drinkable (can be drunk safely)
Discourse analysis and linguistics • Discourse analysis comes from the realization that language, action and knowledge are in separable. J.L Austin pointed out in his 1955 lectures at Harvard University that utterances are actions (Austin, 1962). Some actions can be performed only through language (e.g. apologizing), whilst others can be performed either verbally or non-verbally (e.g. threatening). In addition, when we start to study how language is used in social interaction, it becomes clear that communication is impossible without shared knowledge and assumptions between speakers and hearers.
Discourse analysis and linguistics • There is a dialectic relationship between language and situation. In certain games, ceremonies and formal rituals, actual forms of words may be laid down as part of the proceedings. Of course most everyday uses of language are more flexible. Given a social situation such as a small village shop’, it is possible to predict a great deal about the content, functions and style of language used there. Much of the language will be either local gossip or transactional, concerned with buying and selling. We often know what kind of language to expect in different situations; and conversely, given a fragment of language, we can often reconstruct in some detail the social situation which produced it.
Discourse analysis and linguistics • For example, when we turn the knob of a radio to tune in different programs. It usually takes a few seconds to identify whether we are listening to a sermon, sports commentary, quiz program, news broadcast, play, or to a program for young children or school pupils, or whatever. The identifying features include phonology, lexis, syntax and paralinguistic features.
Discourse analysis and linguistics • Furthermore, situations can be altered or created by using language in different ways, for example, by selecting the speech act of whispering sweet nothings, rather than nagging, carping, complaining or accusing. It is clear enough that much language is not to be taken literally, that language is used to perform actions, and that different social situations produce different language.
Discourse analysis and linguistics • An implication of points about the uses of language is that much of syntactic theory has to be thoroughly reconsidered. An increasing number of linguists are starting to suggest that the analysis of sentence structure should be based on an information structure of a given-new or theme-rheme kind.
Discourse analysis and linguistics • Much of the fascination of discourse analysis derives, in fact, from the realization that the boundaries of linguistics are being redrawn. In has become increasingly clear that a coherent view of language, including syntax, must take account of discourse phenomena. The grammatical, structural units of clause or sentence are not necessarily either the most important nits for language study, or the biggest, although the clause will probably remain basic as a unit of syntax, or prepositional information, and as the potential realization of a speech act.
Discourse analysis and linguistics • It is worthwhile making explicit the implications of discourse analysis for sociolinguistic theory. Sociolinguistics will ultimately have to be based, at least partly, on analysis of how people actually talk to each other in everyday settings, such as streets, shops, restaurants, buses, trains, schools, factories and homes. Therefore, sociolinguistics will have to incorporate analyses of how conversation works. It is chiefly through conversational interaction, social ‘roles’ are recognized and sustained. We can talk about intuitively recognizable social roles such as ‘teacher’ and ‘doctor’. There is quite specific conversational behavior attached to being a teacher.
Discourse analysis and linguistics • On the one hand, there is no use of language which is not embedded in the culture; on the other hand, there are no large-scale relationships between language and society which are not realized, at least partly, through verbal interaction. Part of cultural knowledge is conversational competence.
Discourse organization • By definition, sentence grammars ignore the syntagmatic chaining of clauses and sentences in larger exchanges or sequences. Certain phenomena involving particles, adverbs and conjunctions can only be explained with reference to the syntagmatic chaining of linguistic units at the clause or above. Discourse is a rank above the clause and a level of linguistic description above syntax.
Discourse organization • Firstly, connected discourse is not random. People are quite able to distinguish between a random list of sentences and a coherent text, and it is the principles which underlie this recognition of coherence which are the topic of the study for discourse analysts.
Discourse organization • There are several ways of demonstrating informally that discourse is organized. First, conversationalists themselves frequently refer to discourse structure in the course of conversation, by utterances such as oh, by the way...., anyway, as I was saying .... In everyday situations, people are aware that not anything can follow anything: some utterance require to be prefaced by such an excuse or a claim of relevance. This insertion of metatext, pointing to the organization of the text itself, is particularly common in certain discourse styles, such as lecturing, and it occurs in written and spoken discourse.
Discourse organization • Second, there are many jokes which depend on our ability to recognize faulty discourse sequences. The simplest type is: • (1) A: Yes, I can. • B: Can you see into the future?
Discourse organization • At the risk of being tedious, this joke depends on two things: the recognition that the question-answer sequence has been reversed; and that the grammatical cohesion has been disrupted. Yes, I can is elliptic and only interpretable via the following, instead of the preceding, utterance.
Discourse organization • One the same mode is the joke about the man who goes into a chemist’s shop. The exchange goes: • (2) Customer: Good morning. Do you have anything to treat complete lost of voice? • Shopkeeper: Good morning, sir. And what can I do for you? • In this case, the shopkeeper’s utterance occurs in the wrong structural position. It occurs second, although it is strongly marked as an opening conversational move: by the greeting, the address term, the content and form of the question.
Discourse organization • Such examples demonstrate immediately our discourse competence to recognize that utterance can occur in some sequences but not others. Therefore, discourse should in principle be analyzable in terms of syntagmatic constraints on possible sequences of utterances. Stubbs ( 1983:17) points out that some discourse sequences are impossible, or at least highly improbable. Consider this example: