470 likes | 579 Views
WHAT IS A BIOSTATISTICIAN TRYING TO PROVE? PROOF IN MEDICAL RESEARCH. Susan S. Ellenberg , Ph.D. Department of Biostatistics and Epidemiology Perelman School of Medicine, U Penn. Statistics is the science of uncertainty Biostatistics is the application of statistics to biological problems
E N D
WHAT IS A BIOSTATISTICIAN TRYING TO PROVE?PROOF IN MEDICAL RESEARCH Susan S. Ellenberg, Ph.D. Department of Biostatistics and Epidemiology Perelman School of Medicine, U Penn
Statistics is the science of uncertainty • Biostatistics is the application of statistics to biological problems • Many biostatisticians work in medical research
PROOF IN BIOSTATISTICS • Many biostatisticians develop theory, just as mathematicians do • Proofs usually relate to the validity of methods developed for applications • Analysis of data • Study design • Estimation • Modeling processes
BIOSTATISTICIANS AND PROOF IN MEDICAL RESEARCH • Much of what biostatisticians do is work with other scientists to design and analyze studies to answer questions of interest • The job of the biostatistician is to ensure that the study is designed and analyzed in a way that the results are persuasive and that they can support decision-making • Nota bene: “persuasive” and “can support decision-making” are inherently subjective
TYPES OF DECISIONS • Government decisions • Organizational decisions • Personal decisions
GOVERNMENT DECISIONS • Should a medical treatment be approved for marketing? • Should an approved treatment be withdrawn or subject to more restrictions? • Should certain pesticides be banned? • Should a new vaccine be recommended for widespread use? • Should our community fluoridate its water?
ORGANIZATIONAL DECISIONS • What treatments will an insurance company cover? • What treatment strategies will be recommended by medical societies? • Will a hospital purchase the latest diagnostic scanner?
PERSONAL DECISIONS • Should I undergo surgery for relief of my back pain? • Should I vaccinate my children? • Should I start storing my leftovers in glass instead of plastic containers? • Do I need to use my cell phone less to reduce risk of brain cancer? • Should I keep taking my (antidepressant, painkiller, blood pressure drug, etc)?
PROOF IN MEDICINE • “Proof” in medicine is all about limiting the uncertainty surrounding our inferences from data • Classic approach: if A does not cause B, how likely would we have been to observe the results before us? • Completely analogous to flipping a coin to see if it is “fair;” if we flip a coin 10 times and get 8 heads, we wonder how likely it would be to get 8 or more heads with a fair coin
SMOKING AND LUNG CANCER • British Medical Journal, 1950, Doll and Hill • Smoking habits of hospitalized patients
DID THIS PROVE SMOKING CAUSES LUNG CANCER? • Doll and Hill were pretty convinced • Many others were skeptical • Most men were smokers at that time • People didn’t want to believe that smoking was harmful • Some very eminent scientists at the time argued that there could have been other differences between the two sets of hospitalized patients that led to this apparent association • Maybe people who were predisposed to getting lung cancer were also predisposed to smoke
FUNDAMENTAL PROBLEM WITH OBSERVATIONAL DATA • Can have “association” without “causation” • There could be some other factor that is responsible for the apparent association • Example: the more churches there are in a town, the more bars there are • We call this phenomenon “confounding”
HORMONE REPLACEMENT AND HEART DISEASE • Many observational studies showed a lower rate of heart disease in postmenopausal women taking hormone replacement therapy • Biologically plausible finding • Example: Grodstein et al, 1996
DID THESE STUDIES PROVE THAT HORMONE REPLACEMENT PREVENTS HEART DISEASE? • Many physicians believed they did • A very large proportion of postmenopausal women were prescribed this therapy • But there were skeptics who worried this could be a false association, caused by one or more confounding factors
AN ALTERNATIVE APPROACH • After many years of debate, the National Institutes of Health funded a large experiment—a randomized clinical trial—to study this question • Women were asked to have their treatment (hormones or placebo) decided by a coin flip • When treatment is assigned at random, any confounding factors (both known and unknown) will be equally distributed across treatment groups, so cannot influence the conclusions
NOT AN EASY STUDY TO DO • If most physicians believe hormone therapy will prevent bad disease, why would their patients agree to a 50% chance of getting a placebo? • Some were concerned the study was unethical because half the women would not get a treatment they believed was beneficial
WHAT HAPPENED? • The clinical trial entered over 68,000 women from 1993 through 1998, and followed their health status • In 2002 the main results were announced • The women who received hormone replacement therapy had MORE heart disease than women who received placebo • The observational studies were wrong
WHY WERE THE RESULTS DIFFERENT? • Best guess: despite efforts in the observational studies to account for differences between women who did and did not take hormones, there was probably some remaining “healthy subject” bias • Women who were prescribed hormones were somewhat healthier than women who were not • This may have outweighed the harm caused by the hormone therapy
SMOKING VS HORMONES • Important difference between these cases • One question could be studied in a randomized trial; the other could not • Randomized trials are the best* way we have to “prove” something in medicine but clearly many important questions cannot be studied in this way *”best” is not perfect; we may have multiple randomized trials with conflicting results!
PROOF AND THE RANDOMIZED CLINICAL TRIAL • Give half of the subjects treatment A and half treatment B • Observe the number of successes for A and B • Calculate how likely these numbers would be if A and B truly had the same chance of producing a success
PROOF AND THE RANDOMIZED CLINICAL TRIAL Expected outcomes (no treatment effect) Observed outcomes
PROOF AND THE RANDOMIZED CLINICAL TRIAL • A biostatistician will help design a trial so that the results will be considered “proof” • The trial will have to be big enough so that, if you observe a difference at least as large as you think you will, you will be able to dismiss the possibility that the difference is just due to chance • We make sure that the amount of uncertainty about the result is small
WHAT WE CAN STUDY IN A RANDOMIZED TRIAL • Effectiveness and safety of medical treatments • Whether one treatment is preferable to another • Approaches to disease prevention (harder) • Smoking cessation programs • Diets • Exercise programs
WHAT WE CAN’T STUDY IN A RANDOMIZED TRIAL • Effects of environmental exposures
WHAT WE CAN’T STUDY IN A RANDOMIZED TRIAL • Effects of environmental exposures • Effects of inherent individual characteristics • Implications of genetic characteristics
WHAT WE CAN’T STUDY IN A RANDOMIZED TRIAL • Effects of environmental exposures • Effects of inherent individual characteristics • Implications of genetic characteristics • Effects of “lifestyle choices” • Amount of alcohol consumed • Level and /or type of sexual activity
WHAT WE CAN’T STUDY IN A RANDOMIZED TRIAL • Effects of environmental exposures • Effects of inherent individual characteristics • Implications of genetic characteristics • Effects of “lifestyle choices” • Amount of alcohol consumed • Level and /or type of sexual activity • Treatments for extremely rare conditions
WHAT WE CAN’T STUDY IN A RANDOMIZED TRIAL • Effects of environmental exposures • Effects of inherent individual characteristics • Implications of genetic characteristics • Effects of “lifestyle choices” • Amount of alcohol consumed • Level and /or type of sexual activity • Treatments for extremely rare conditions • Potential harms of a treatment with known important benefits
CONFOUNDING FACTORS • We attempt to study such questions using observational data but have to contend with confounding • Alcohol consumption shows an association with lung cancer, but heavy drinkers are more likely to be smokers than occasional or non-drinkers • Environmental exposures can be confounded with socioeconomic status, which affects where people live and work, what they eat, etc. • Many biostatisticians focus their research on methods to reduce the effect of confounding factors on the association
BIG ISSUE: VACCINE SAFETY • Virtually every child receives multiple vaccines at multiple times, starting at or soon after birth • Just about every bad thing that happens to a baby or a child follows the initiation of vaccinations • It is understandable that parents of a child diagnosed with a serious illness or condition shortly after one or more vaccinations would wonder if the vaccines caused the problem
VACCINES AND AUTISM • The rate of autism diagnosis has increased markedly • The number of vaccines given to children has increased markedly • Is there a connection? Can we prove it one way or the other? • Could we do a randomized trial to see if vaccinated children were less likely to become autistic?
THE DIFFICULTY • Because of the clinical trials that have been done we know that vaccines are very effective at preventing serious diseases • It would be unethical to conduct a study that left some children unprotected • Since nearly all children are vaccinated, and those who are not are different in important ways from those who are, even observational studies are difficult to do
THE DIFFICULTY • Because of the clinical trials that have been done we know that vaccines are very effective at preventing serious diseases • It would be unethical to conduct a study that left some children unprotected • Since nearly all children are vaccinated, and those who are not are different in important ways from those who are, even observational studies are difficult to do • (Note: many studies assessing specific vaccines have been done and have not shown any association of these vaccines with autism)
PROVING A NEGATIVE • Often we are interested in demonstrating that there is NO association between an exposure and an outcome • We might want to show that a lower dose of a drug is just as effective as a higher dose • We might want to show that a new treatment that might be safer, or more convenient to take, or cheaper, is just as good as the standard treatment • We might want to show (as in the vaccine example) that a particular treatment, or exposure, does not have an adverse effect.
PROVING A NEGATIVE • Even when a clinical trial is possible, it is more complicated than when we want to show that an effect is GREATER THAN zero • Consider the example of lower vs higher dose • We don’t want to use the lower dose if it’s worse • We don’t think that the lower dose can be better • We know that the difference can’t be exactly zero
SO WHAT DO WE DO? • We recognize that “no worse” has to be stretched to mean “no more than a LITTLE BIT worse” • We design our study so that the probability that the lower dose will appear more than a little bit worse is very small, if in fact it is truly NO worse • If we think the standard dose will be successful 75% of the time, we might design our study so that, if the low dose is as good as the standard dose, it won’t appear more than 5% worse
MORE UNCERTAINTY • If a treatment is successful 75% of the time in one study, how likely is it to be successful at least 75% of the time in another study? • Easy to calculate, if the people in the second study are just like the people in the first study… • Or, if we can assume that even if people in the second study are NOT like people in the first study, the differences won’t affect the chance that the treatment will be successful… • Or, even if the differences will affect the chance of success, we can use a statistical model that will adjust for these differences (if we know what they are)
ANOTHER COMPONENT OF PROOF IN MEDICINE • Biology is important • In some cases, what we know about the biology makes all the difference • Do we need a randomized trial to prove that a particular food or drug can cause a serious allergic reaction? • Could we prove that homeopathy “works” by doing a randomized trial?
HOMEOPATHY • Homeopathy is an “alternative” approach to medical treatment that is based on the concept that small doses of a substance that causes disease symptoms will effectively treat that disease • Practitioners take such a substance and dilute it down so that in many cases it is impossible to find even one molecule of the substance in the dose given to a patient • The dilutant is supposed to retain the memory of the original substance
HOMEOPATHY • Most medical practitioners dismiss the concept of homeopathy on scientific grounds • If presented with a randomized clinical trial that yielded a positive result they would not change their belief • They would assume the trial had not been conducted properly • Healthier patients had been given the homeopathy • Measurement of the outcome had been influenced by knowledge of who was taking which treatment • Fraud
MANY OTHER ASPECTS TO “PROOF” • There are many ways to make results look stronger than they should • If your study shows that your drug doesn’t work, find a subset of the study population in which it DOES appear to work (surprisingly easy) • If you don’t find the effect you were looking for, look for some other effect—if you look hard enough you’ll find one (data dredging) • When we consider study results we want to make sure we’re not being had
ALL IS NOT CONFUSION • There are many things we do know • Vaccines prevent disease • Antibiotics cure infections • Drugs and radiation can shrink tumors • Epinephrine can diminish allergic responses • Antiviral drugs can control HIV replication and prevent transmission of HIV from mother to child • Exposure to lead is bad for developing brains • Chronic exposure to asbestos promotes mesothelioma • Certain genetic characteristics predispose to breast cancer
A leading medical researcher once called statisticians the “terrorists of medical research” • Clinical investigators don’t always appreciate being told that their findings could be due to chance, or that the analysis they want to do is invalid • Our job is to avoid pitfalls, and develop improved methods of design and analysis • There is always some residual uncertainty
TO SUM UP • Proof in medical research is subjective • Even widely accepted beliefs are questioned by some • HIV is the cause of AIDS • The best we can do is to improve our understanding of the underlying biology, and design and conduct studies that minimize the probability we will draw erroneous conclusions • We have to be open to the possibility that someone will “disprove” what we have just “proven”