760 likes | 773 Views
Explore the relationship between the Necker Cube and triangle nodes in the context of abstract neurons, and how they encode relational information. Learn about parallel activation streams, top-down and bottom-up processes, and mutual inhibition between structures. Discover the connectionist model of word recognition and its application in computational neurobiology.
E N D
o u t p u t y { 1 if net > 0 0 otherwise w0 i0=1 w1 w2 wn . . . i1 i2 in i n p u t i Abstract Neuron
Constrained Best Fit in Nature inanimate animate
Triangle Nodes: Encoding relational information with abstract neurons • The triangle node (aka 2/3 node) is a useful function that activates its outputs (3) if any (2) of its 3 inputs are active • Such a node will be useful for lots of representations.
Triangle nodes and McCullough-Pitts Neurons Relation (A) Object (B) Value (C) A B C
Basic Ideas • Parallel activation streams. • Top down and bottom up activation combine to determine the best matching structure. • Triangle nodes bind features of objects to values • Mutual inhibition and competition between structures • Mental connections are active neural connections
5levels of Neural Theory of Language Pyscholinguistic experiments Spatial Relation Motor Control Metaphor Grammar Cognition and Language Computation Structured Connectionism abstraction Neural Net SHRUTI Computational Neurobiology Triangle Nodes Biology Neural Development Quiz Midterm Finals
Behavioral Experiments • Identity – Mental activity is Structured Neural Activity • Spreading Activation— Psychological model/theory behind priming and interference experiments • Simulation — Necessary for meaningfulness and contextual inference • Parameters — Govern simulation, strict inference, link to language
Bottom-up vs. Top-down Processes • Bottom-up: When processing is driven by the stimulus • Top-down: When knowledge and context are used to assist and drive processing • Interaction: The stimulus is the basis of processing but almost immediately top-down processes are initiated
Stroop Effect • Interference between form and meaning
Name the words BookCarTableBoxTrashManBed CornSitPaperCoin Glass HouseJar KeyRugCatDoll Letter BabyTomato CheckPhone Soda DishLampWoman
Name the print color of the words BlueGreenRed YellowOrangeBlackRed PurpleGreenRedBlueYellowBlackRed GreenWhiteBlueYellow Red BlackBlue WhiteRed Yellow GreenBlackPurple
Body-Specificity Hypothesis If concepts and word meanings are constituted, in part, by mental simulations our own perceptions and actions… …then their neurocognitive representations should differ for people with different kinds of bodies, who perceive and act upon the environment in systematically different ways. (Casasanto, in review, link on course page)
Testing body-specificity pinch chew Manual Action Non-Manual Action
Testing body-specificity Left handed movements for one block of trials Right handed movements for the other block of trials
Design • 96 Words (48 manual verbs, 48 non-manual verbs) • 2 blocks (LH movements, RH movements) • 2 groups of Ss (Left handers, Right handers) • Dependent measures: RT & Surprise recognition (Old/New)
Move marbles by wordcolor caress jab fling grip pound tap yank erase dial sigh cough growl watch crawl peek tumble dance say Manual Action Non-Manual Action
Predictions If action word meanings are constituted, in part, by mental simulation of perceptuo-motor experiences, then: Both online and offline effects of congruity should be found between manual motor actions and the meanings of manual action verbs (but not non-manual action verbs). Right and left handed participants should show opposite effects of using their right and left hands to move marbles during incidental encoding of manual action verbs. (Casasanto, in review)
ns ns Non-Manual Action Verb ReactionTime Results *** *** Marble movement time in ms Manual Action Verb (Casasanto, in review)
Congruity effect in ms ABS Laterality Quotient Handedness-ness predicts congruity effect
ns ns Non-Manual Action Verb Recognition Memory Results LeftHand 0.80 RightHand 0.75 *** *** 0.70 Proportion correct recognition 0.65 0.60 0.55 0.50 LEFTIES RIGHTIES Manual Action Verb (Casasanto, in review)
Procedure for experiment that demonstrates the word-superiority effect. First the word is presented, then the mask XXXX’s, then the letters.
Word-Superiority Effect Reicher (1969) • Which condition resulted in faster & more accurate recognition of the letter? • The word condition • Letters are recognized faster when they are part of a word then when they are alone • This rejects the completely bottom-up feature model • Also a challenge for serial processing
Connectionist ModelMcClelland & Rumelhart (1981) • Knowledge is distributed and processing occurs in parallel, with both bottom-up and top-down influences • This model can explain the Word-Superiority Effect because it can account for context effects
Basic Ideas • Parallel activation streams. • Top down and bottom up activation combine to determine the best matching structure. • Triangle nodes bind features of objects to values • Mutual inhibition and competition between structures • Mental connections are active neural connections
Interaction in language processing: Pragmatic constraints on lexical access Jim Magnuson Columbia University
Information integration • A central issue in psycholinguistics and cognitive science: • When/how are such sources integrated? • Two views • Interaction • Use information as soon as it is available • Free flow between levels of representation • Modularity • Protect and optimize levels by encapsulation • Staged serial processing • Reanalyze / appeal to top-down information only when needed
Reaction Times in Milliseconds after: “They all rose” 0 delay 200ms. delay
Example: Modularity and word recognition • Tanenhaus et al. (1979) [also Swinney, 1979] • Given a homophone likerose, and a context biased towards one sense, when is context integrated? • Spoken sentence primes ending in homophones: • They all rose vs. They bought a rose • Secondary task: name a displayed orthographic word • Probe at offset of ambiguous word: priming for both“stood” and “flower” • 200 ms later: only priming for appropriate sense • Suggests encapsulation followed by rapid integration • But the constraint here is weak -- overestimates modularity? • How could we examine strong constraints in natural contexts?
“They all rose” triangle nodes: when two of the abstract neurons fire, the third also fires model of spreading activation
Allopenna, Magnuson & Tanenhaus (1998) Eye Eye camera tracking computer Scene camera ‘Pick up the beaker’
TRACE predictions Do rhymes compete? • Cohort (Marlsen-Wilson): onset similarity is primary because of the incremental nature of speech (serial/staged; Shortlist/Merge) • Cat activates cap, cast, cattle, camera, etc. • Rhymes won’t compete • NAM (Neighborhood Activation Model; Luce): global similarity is primary • Cat activatesbat, rat, cot, cast, etc. • Rhymes among set of strong competitors • TRACE (McClelland & Elman): global similarity constrained by incremental nature of speech • Cohorts and rhymes compete, but with different time course
Study 1 Conclusions • As predicted by interactive models, cohorts and rhymes are activated, with different time courses • Eye movement paradigm • More sensitive than conventional paradigms • More naturalistic • Simultaneous measures of multiple items • Transparently linkable to computational model • Time locked to speech at a fine grain
Theoretical conclusions • Natural contexts provide strong constraints that are used • When those constraints are extremely predictive, they are integrated as quickly as we can measure • Suggests rapid, continuous interaction among • Linguistic levels • Nonlinguistic context • Even for processes assumed to be low-level and automatic • Constrains processing theories, also has implications for, e.g., learnability
Producing words from pictures or from other words: A comparison of aphasic lexical access from two different input modalities Gary Dell with Myrna Schwartz, Dan Foygel, Nadine Martin, Eleanor Saffran, Deborah Gagnon, Rick Hanley, Janice Kay, Susanne Gahl, Rachel Baron, Stefanie Abel, Walter Huber
Boxes and arrows in the linguistic system Semantics Syntax Lexicon Output Phonology Input Phonology
Picture Naming Task Semantics Say: “cat” Syntax Lexicon Output Phonology Input Phonology
A 2-step Interactive Model of Lexical Access in Production Semantic Features FOG DOG CAT RAT MAT f r d k m ae o t g Onsets Vowels Codas
Step 1 – Lemma Access Activate semantic features of CAT FOG DOG CAT RAT MAT f r d k m ae o t g Onsets Vowels Codas
Step 1 – Lemma Access Activation spreads through network FOG DOG CAT RAT MAT f r d k m ae o t g Onsets Vowels Codas
Step 1 – Lemma Access Most active word from proper category is selected and linked to syntactic frame NP N FOG DOG CAT RAT MAT f r d k m ae o t g Onsets Vowels Codas
Step 2 – Phonological Access Jolt of activation is sent to selected word NP N FOG DOG CAT RAT MAT f r d k m ae o t g Onsets Vowels Codas
Step 2 – Phonological Access Activation spreads through network NP N FOG DOG CAT RAT MAT f r d k m ae o t g Onsets Vowels Codas
Step 2 – Phonological Access Most activated phonemes are selected FOG DOG CAT RAT MAT Syl On Vo Co f r d k m ae o t g Onsets Vowels Codas
Semantic Error – “dog” Shared features activate semantic neighbors NP N FOG DOG CAT RAT MAT f r d k m ae o t g Onsets Vowels Codas