1 / 16

Chapter 4: More on Two Variable Data

Chapter 4: More on Two Variable Data. Sec. 4.2 – Cautions about Correlation and Regression. Cautions about Correlation and Regression. Recall from chapter 3: T hat correlation and regression describe only linear relationships That c orrelation and the LSRL are not resistant

Download Presentation

Chapter 4: More on Two Variable Data

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Chapter 4: More on Two Variable Data Sec. 4.2 – Cautions about Correlation and Regression

  2. Cautions about Correlation and Regression • Recall from chapter 3: • That correlation and regression describe only linear relationships • That correlation and the LSRL are not resistant • One influential point or incorrectly entered data point can completely change the data. • Always plot your data before interpreting regression or correlation

  3. Extrapolation • Extrapolation is the use of a regression line far outside the domain of values of the explanatory variable x that you used to obtain the line or curve. • Such predictions are not accurate • Example • Suppose that you have data on a child’s growth between the years 3 and 8. You find a strong linear relationship between age x and height y. If you fit a regression line to these data and use it to predict the child’s height at 25 years old you would predict them to be 8 feet tall • Don’t stray far from the domain of x that actually appears in your data

  4. Lurking Variables • Sometimes the relationship between two variables is influenced by other variables that we did not measure or even think about • A lurking variable is a variable that is not among the explanatory or response variables in study and yet may influence the interpretation of relationships among those variables. • The relationship between two variables can be strongly influenced by lurking variables. • A lurking variable can falsely suggest a strong relationship between x and y or it can hide a relationship that is really there.

  5. Lurking Variables • Because lurking variables are often unrecognized and unmeasured, detecting their effect is a challenge • Many lurking variables change systematically over time. • One method of detecting if time has an influence is to plot residuals and response variables against the time order if available.

  6. Lurking Variables

  7. The Question of Causation • In many studies of the relationship between two variables, the goal is to establish that changes in the explanatory variable cause changes in the response variable. • Even when a strong association is present, the conclusion that this association is due to a causal linking in the variables is often elusive.

  8. Explaining Association • Strong Associations can generally be explained by one of three relationships. • 1. Causation • 2. Common Response • 3. Confounding • Variable x and y show a strong association (dashed line). This association may be the result of any of several causal relationships (solid arrow).

  9. Causation: • x causes y • Confounding: • x may cause y, but y may instead be caused by a confounding variable z • CommonResponse: • x and y are reacting to a lurking variable z Explaining Association

  10. Causation • Causation is not easily established. • The best evidence for causation comes from experiments that change x while holding all other factors fixed. • Even a very strong association between two variables is not by itself good evidence that there is a cause-and-effect link between the variables.

  11. Examples of Direct Causation • The following relationships are examples of direct causation, but “causation” is not a simple idea. • Refer to p.233 for explanations 1. x =mother’s BMI y= daughter’s BMI 2. x = amount of saccharin in a rat’s diet y = count of tumors in the rat’s bladder

  12. Common Response • Beware of lurking variables when thinking about an association between two variables. • The observed association between the variables x and y is explained by a lurking variable z. Both x and y change to changes in z. • This common response creates an association even though there may be no direct causal link between x and y.

  13. Examples of common Response • The following relationships are examples of how common response can create an association. • Refer to p.233 for explanations 3. x = a high school senior’s SAT score y = the student’s first-year college GPA 4. x = monthly flow of money into stock mutual funds y = monthly rate of return for the stock market

  14. Confounding • Two variables areconfoundedwhen their effects on a response variable cannot be distinguished from each other. The confounded variables may be either explanatory variables or lurking variables. • Confounding of several variables often prevents us from drawing conclusions about causation.

  15. Examples of Confounding • The following relationships are examples of confounding • Refer to p.234 for explanations 5. x = whether a person regularly attends religious services y = how long the person lives 6. x = the number of years of education a worker has y = the worker’s income

  16. Homework: p.237-239 #’s 33-36, 38 & 41

More Related