1 / 67

Self-Justification

Self-Justification. Chapter Five. Self-Justification. People are motivated to justify their own actions, beliefs, and feelings. When they do something, they will try, if at all possible, to convince themselves (and others) that it was a logical, reasonable thing to do.

oberle
Download Presentation

Self-Justification

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Self-Justification Chapter Five

  2. Self-Justification • People are motivated to justify their own actions, beliefs, and feelings. • When they do something, they will try, if at all possible, to convince themselves (and others) that it was a logical, reasonable thing to do. • Example: Schachter & Singer experiment with epinephrine injections from Chapter 2

  3. Self-Justification • The concept of self-justification can be applied very broadly. • Example: Disasters and rumor-spreading (Prasad; Sinha)

  4. Self-Justification • Festinger organized research findings on self-justification and developed a theory of human motivation he called cognitive dissonance. • Cognitive dissonance is a state of tension that occurs whenever an individual simultaneously holds two cognitions (ideas, attitudes, beliefs, opinions) that are psychologically inconsistent.

  5. Self-Justification • To hold two ideas that contradict each other is to flirt with absurdity, so how do we convince ourselves that our lives are not absurd? • That is, how do we reduce cognitive dissonance? • By changing one or both cognitions in such a way as to render them more compatible (more consonant) with each other by adding more cognitions that help bridge the gap between the original cognitions • Example: Smoking and cancer warnings

  6. Self-Justification • The theory of cognitive dissonance does not picture people as rational beings; rather, it pictures them as rationalizing being. • According to the underlying assumptions of the theory, we humans are motivated not so much to BE right as to BELIEVE we are right.

  7. Self-Justification • Sometimes, our motivation to be right and our motivation to believe we are right work in the same direction. • Occasionally, however, the need to reduce dissonance leads to behavior that is maladaptive and therefore irrational. • Example: Gibbons, et al. smoking study

  8. Self-Justification • One way to reduce dissonance and regain a healthy sense of self is to trivialize one’s commitment to a failed goal. • Example: New Year’s resolutions • An alternative (and potentially less maladaptive) response is to lower one’s expectations for success.

  9. Self-Justification • This ties-in with information on attitude change from Chapter 3. • If people are committed to an attitude, the information a communicator presents arouses dissonance. • Frequently, the best way to reduce the dissonance is to reject or distort the evidence. • The deeper a person’s commitment to an attitude, the greater his or her tendency to reject dissonant evidence. • Example: Hale-Bopp suicides • Example: Princeton-Dartmouth football game (Hastrof & Cantril)

  10. Self-Justification • People don’t like to see or hear things that conflict with their deeply held beliefs or wishes. • Ancient response? Kill the messenger! • Modern-day figurative response is to blame the media for presentation of material that produces the pain of dissonance. • Example: Political candidate coverage and letters to the editor

  11. Dissonance Reduction and Rational Behavior • Dissonance-reducing behavior is “irrational” in that it often is maladaptive. • It can prevent people from learning important facts or from finding real solutions to their problems. • On the other hand, it does serve a purpose: • Dissonance-reducing behavior is ego-defensive behavior. • By reducing dissonance, we maintain a positive image or ourselves.

  12. Dissonance Reduction and Rational Behavior • The irrationality of dissonance-reducing behavior has been amply demonstrated. • Example: Jones & Kohler studies on racial segregation • Example: Lord, Ross & Lepper study on capital punishment • The dissonance process probably accounts for the fact that, on issues like politics and religion, people who are deeply committed will almost never come to see things our way, no matter how powerful and balanced our arguments are.

  13. Dissonance Reduction and Rational Behavior • It is important to note that some people are able to tolerate dissonance better than others, but that we are all capable of rational behavior and dissonance-reducing behavior, depending on the circumstance.

  14. Dissonance as a Consequence of Making a Decision • Following a decision, especially a difficult one or one that involves significant effort, people almost always experience dissonance. • This is so because the chosen alternative is seldom entirely positive and the rejected alternative is seldom entirely negative. • A good way to reduce such dissonance is to seek out entirely positive information about the choice you made and avoid negative information about it. • Example: Ehrlich, et al. study of advertising readership

  15. Dissonance as a Consequence of Making a Decision • To reduce dissonance, people cognitively spread apart the alternatives. • That is, after making a decision, people emphasize the positive attributes of the choice they made while deemphasizing the negative attributes. • Similarly, for the choice they did not make, people emphasize the negative attributes and deemphasize the positive attributes. • Example: Brehm study with appliances and ratings

  16. Dissonance as a Consequence of Making a Decision • The tendency to justify one’s choices is not limited to consumer decisions. • Research has demonstrated that similar processes can even affect our romantic relationships and our willingness to consider becoming involved with alternative partners. • Example: Johnson & Rusbult • Example: Simpson, et al.

  17. Dissonance as a Consequence of Making a Decision • In sum, whether we are talking about appliances or romantic partners, once a firm commitment has been made, people tend to focus on the positive aspects of their choices and to downplay the attractive qualities of the unchosen alternatives.

  18. Dissonance as a Consequence of Making a Decision • It is impossible to overstate the potential dangers posed by our susceptibility to these tendencies. • History gives us a number of frightening and disturbing examples: • Jews in Sighet, Hungary • Escalation of the war in Vietnam (White) • George W. Bush, Hussein, and WMD

  19. Dissonance as a Consequence of Making a Decision • How can a skilled leader avoid falling into the self-justification trap? • Historical examples show us that the way out of this process is for leaders to bring in skilled advisors outside the inner circle because they are not caught up in the need to reduce dissonance from their earlier decision-making.

  20. Dissonance as a Consequence of Making a Decision • The process underlying escalation has been explored, on a more individual level, under controlled experimental conditions. • Example: Freedman & Fraser

  21. Dissonance as a Consequence of Making a Decision • Using small favors to encourage people to accede to larger requests has been dubbed the “foot-in-the-door technique.” • It is effective because having done the smaller favor sets up pressure toward agreeing to do the larger favor. • Example: Pliner, et al. • Example: Milgram study of obedience

  22. The Importance of Irrevocability • One of the important characteristics seen in examples of cognitive dissonance is the relative irrevocability of the decision. • Tentative decisions are not likely to create significant cognitive conflict. • Some direct evidence of the importance of irrevocability comes from a study by Knox & Inskter of the cognitive gyrations of gamblers at a race track. • Certainty of winning was higher in those who had just placed their bets as compared with those on the way to betting. • Another example: Gilbert

  23. The Importance of Irrevocability • Although the irrevocability of a decision always increases dissonance and the motivation to reduce it, there are circumstances in which irrevocability is unnecessary. • Example: Lowballing (Cialdini)

  24. The Importance of Irrevocability • Dissonance can also impact the decision to behave morally or immorally. • Example: Making the decision to (not) cheat (Mills)

  25. The Importance of Irrevocability • The conclusion? People who almost decide to live in glass houses are frequently the ones who are most prone to throw stones.

  26. The Importance of Irrevocability • It should be clear that the same mechanism that enables a person to cling to an attitude can induce that individual to change an attitude. • It depends on which course of action will serve most to reduce dissonance under the circumstances.

  27. The Importance of Irrevocability • A person who understands the theory can set up the proper condition to induce attitude change in other people by making them vulnerable to certain kinds of beliefs: • If you want people to form more positive attitudes towards an object, get them to commit themselves to own that object. • If you want people to soften their moral attitudes toward some misdeed, tempt them to perform that deed. • If you want people to harden their moral attitudes toward a misdeed, tempt them – but not enough to induce them to commit the deed.

  28. The Psychology of Inadequate Justification • Attitude change as a means of reducing dissonance is not limited to postdecision situations. • It can occur in countless other contexts: • Some contexts require external justification to reduce dissonance. • Examples: Avoid hurt feelings, drunkenness, money • Other contexts require internal justification to reduce dissonance.

  29. The Psychology of Inadequate Justification • If an individual states a belief that is difficult to justify externally, that person will attempt to justify it internally by making his or her attitudes more consistent with the statement.

  30. The Psychology of Inadequate Justification • Dissonance theory predicts that we begin to believe our own lies – but only if there is not abundant external justification for making the statements that run counter to our original attitudes. • This has been called the “saying is believing” paradigm.

  31. The Psychology of Inadequate Justification • This concept also is tied to conformity: • The greater the reward for compliance, the greater the probability that a person will comply. • When it comes to producing a lasting change in attitude, the greater the reward, the less likely any attitude change will occur. • In other words, if we change our attitudes because we have made a public statement for minimal external justification, our attitude change will be relatively permanent – we will have succeeded in convincing ourselves that our previous attitudes were incorrect.

  32. The Psychology of Inadequate Justification • These speculations have been investigated scientifically in several experiments: • Example: Festinger & Carlsmith • Example: Cohen • Example: Leippe & Eisenstadt study of counter-attitudinal advocacy

  33. The Psychology of Inadequate Justification What constitutes external justification? Example: Eating a fried grasshopper (Zimbardo, et al.) What is inadequate justification? Example: Festinger & Carlsmith findings on lying versus Mill’s data on cheating

  34. The Psychology of Inadequate Justification • Dissonance and the Self-Concept • Aronson reformulated Festinger’s original theory to focus more attention on the way people conceive of themselves. • This reformulation suggests that dissonance is most powerful in situations in which the self-concept is threatened. • Worse that “I have misled people” than “I said X”

  35. The Psychology of Inadequate Justification • Dissonance and the Self-Concept • The reformulation is based on the assumption that most individuals like to think of themselves as decent people who wouldn’t ordinarily mislead someone.

  36. The Psychology of Inadequate Justification • Dissonance and the Self-Concept • Certain circumstances make this type of dissonance more powerful and have been investigated empirically. • Example: Nel, Helmreich, & Aronson • Lying produces greater attitude change when the liar is undercompensated for lying, especially when the lie is likely to evoke a change in the audience’s belief or behavior.

  37. The Psychology of Inadequate Justification • Dissonance and the Self-Concept • Further research supports a general principle about dissonance and self-concept: • Dissonance effects are greatest when (1) people feel personally responsible for their actions, and (2) their actions have serious consequences. • That is, the greater the consequence and the greater our responsibility for it, the greater the dissonance. • The greater the dissonance, the greater our own attitude change.

  38. The Psychology of Inadequate Justification • Dissonance and the Self-Concept • The notion that dissonance is aroused whenever the self-concept is challenged has some interesting ramifications: • Example: “Even a penny will help” (Cialdini & Schroeder)

  39. The Psychology of Inadequate Justification • Inadequate Rewards as Applied to Education • The insufficient-reward phenomenon applies to all forms of behavior. • Reminder: Performance of a dull task for little pay is rated as more enjoyable than if payment for the task is large. • Example: Elementary school classroom • Example: Deci, et al.; Lepper • By offering children a reward for playing, the experimenters succeeded in turning play into work!

  40. The Psychology of Inadequate Justification • Inadequate Rewards as Applied to Education • What is the impact of praise as a reward? • Henderlong & Lepper reviewed a host of studies and found that praise can be beneficial but only if it is done in moderation and in a way that makes children feel competent. • Causing a person to focus on the extrinsic reasons for performing well will reduce the attractiveness of the task itself. • Additionally, Dweck’s work shows that praise is most effective if it is focused on efforts rather than talent or ability.

  41. The Psychology of Inadequate Justification • Insufficient Punishment • The use of threats of harsh punishment as a means of getting someone to refrain from doing something he or she enjoys necessitates constant harassment and vigilance. • It would be much more efficient and would require much less noxious restraint if, somehow, people could enjoy doing those things that contribute to their own health and welfare – and to the health and welfare of others.

  42. The Psychology of Inadequate Justification • Insufficient Punishment • Allowing people the opportunity to construct their own internal justification can be a large step toward helping them develop a permanent set of values. • Example: Nursery school toy preference (Aronson & Carlsmith; Freedman)

  43. The Justification of Effort • Dissonance theory leads to the prediction that, if a person works hard to attain a goal, that goal will be more attractive than it will be to someone who achieves the same goal with little or no effort. • Example: Aronson & Mills • Example: Gerard & Mathewson

  44. The Justification of Effort • If a person goes through a difficult or painful experience in order to attain some goal or object, that goal or object becomes more attractive. • The process is called justification of effort. • Example: Sapolsky and placebo effect

  45. The Justification of Effort • In most dissonant situations, there is more than one way to reduce dissonance. • Example: Conway & Ross • One way for people to get what they want is to revise what they had.

  46. The Justification of Cruelty • One variation on the theme of dissonance looks at cognitive justification of cruel behavior. • When we act in an unkind or cruel manner toward someone who does not deserve such treatment, we tend to devalue the victim of our actions as “justification.” • Example: Kent State students • Example: Khrushchev and Beria

  47. The Justification of Cruelty • This phenomenon has been demonstrated empirically: • Example: Davis & Jones study • Example: Glass study • It is precisely because I think I am such a nice person that, if I do something that causes you pain, I must convince myself you are a rat!

  48. The Justification of Cruelty • There are circumstances that limit the generality of this phenomenon: • Low self-esteem • The capacity of the victim to retaliate • Example: Berscheid, et al.

  49. The Justification of Cruelty • Study results suggest that, during a war, soldiers might have a greater need to derogate civilian victims than military victims. • Example: Lt. Calley and My Lai • Need to “dehumanize” victims

  50. The Justification of Cruelty • In the final analysis, people are accountable for their own actions. • It is, however, important to acknowledge that certain situational factors can exert a very powerful impact upon human actions. • It is important to understand those factors (cognitive dissonance being one) in the hopes that we can, eventually, avoid them. • One relevant issue is the self-fulfilling prophecy. • It provides a perfect justification for cruelty and neglect.

More Related