1 / 25

Cause in fact

Cause in fact Ford v. Trident Fisheries (Mass. 1919) p. 299 How did P’s decedent die? Alleged negligence of trawler’s owners? lifeboat was lashed to deck; delayed rescue only one oar; had to scull Why a directed verdict for D? The court’s reasoning

oshin
Download Presentation

Cause in fact

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Cause in fact

  2. Ford v. Trident Fisheries (Mass. 1919) p. 299 • How did P’s decedent die? • Alleged negligence of trawler’s owners? • lifeboat was lashed to deck; delayed rescue • only one oar; had to scull • Why a directed verdict for D?

  3. The court’s reasoning • “there is nothing to show they in any way contributed to Ford’s death….that if the boat had been suspended from davits and a different method of propelling it had been used he would have been rescued” • why does the court reach that conclusion? • No idea where Ford was; he was never seen; no cry heard; no clothing seen; never surfaced? • so no reason to believe would have been saved by a speedier craft (Burden of Proof = P)

  4. The “But For” Test of C-I-F • Ford’s analysis =called “but for” test • P must prove: • But for the defendant’s failure to behave reasonably, P would have avoided injury (or suffered less severe injury). • Recall Ford: • “there is nothing to show.. .that if the boat had been suspended from davits and a different method of propelling it had been used he would have been rescued”

  5. Status of the “but for” test • “But for” = universal starting point for c-I-f • If P cannot prove “but for” causation, then loses unless qualifies for an exception • “But for” phrasing=may seem awkward • try this: “if only” D has taken more precautions, P would not have been injured.

  6. Applying the “but for” test • Brian Dailey pulled the chair. But for? • Bargee away from barge? • Failure to insulate power line? • Was Washington’s decision to move his citizens band radio also a “but for” cause? • Having a “rabbit” radio promotion? • Was the driving of the teenage defendants also a “but for” cause? • What if we don’t know who forced P off road?

  7. Relationship to the RS “Substantial Factor” test • The RS does not use “but for” language. • D’s negligence must be a “substantial factor” in P’s injuries. • BUT “substantial factor” is defined to mean that the P prove either: • But for, or • Exception.

  8. Hoyt v. Jeffers (Mich. 1874)p102 • What’s the point of the case? • Causation can be proven using circumstantial evidence. • What if the fire marshal concluded that the fire started in the kitchen? • If we rule out the kitchen or cigars, etc., because the fire started on the roof, what does that do to the odds it was D? • B/P=“more probable than not”

  9. Smith v. Rapid Transit, Inc. (mass. 1945) p. 105 • Facts? • What circumstantial evidence of caustion? • Outcome? • Why not adequate to go to jury? • “not enough that mathematically the chances somewhat favor the proposition” • need “actual belief in its truth”

  10. Probabilistic Proof • Often said the “mere” probabilistic proof is insufficient, standing alone, to create a jury question. • Example: Rapid Transit and Slow Transit are only two companies in isolated town. RT runs 97% of the miles. P is hit by unknown bus. • Prima facie case against RT? (probably RT?) NO. • contrast Hoyt. Circumstantial evidence leads to conclusion that it probably D. [“odds are”!!]

  11. Probabilistic Proof • Pointer #1: several exceptions (coming) • Pointer #2: often hard to apply • what if P knows the bus was blue, like RT? • Blue Mercedes bus? • Blue Mercedes bus with Asian driver? • NB: at some point, no longer just “background odds” but sufficiently particularized to go to jury. (e.g. cracked windshield) • Editorial: No clear line because all circumstantial proof is ultimately “probabilistic”.

  12. Recap • 1. “but for” test--be able to apply • 2. sufficiency of the evidence • circumstantial=allowed • purely probabilistic proof=usually insufficient • 3. NEXT: toxic torts

  13. Cause-in-Fact #2 Toxic Torts

  14. The causation problem in toxic torts. • 1. Ordinary tort: P is negligently hit by a bus. Sues D. Did one of D’s buses cause P’s bad back? • 1. Whose bus negligently hit P? (Smith) • 2. Did the bus impact cause the injuries of which Plaintiff complains? (“but for”) • Example: Pre-existing back problem?

  15. The problem with toxic torts #2 • 2. Toxic tort. P claims that his lung cancer was caused by exposure to Toxifam, an industrial cleaning solvent. • A. Who made the Toxifam-containing solvents used by P’s employer? (Smith) • See “market share” liability puzzles later • B. Does it cause lung cancer? [NEW ISSUE] • C. Is it the most likely cause of P’s lung cancer? (“but for”) [COMPLEX]

  16. Does it cause lung cancer? • P’s typically use medical studies • field=“epidemiology” • Findings must be “statistically significant” • What cause of error in the study does this guard against? • The risk that random chance resulted in an atypical sample being used in the study • Ex: bowl of 10,000 marbles is 70% black. If we pull

  17. Example • Town of Pleasant has 10,000 who voted for Dummy and 8,000 who voted for Liar. • Exit pollers want to know who won before votes are counted. • Decide to “sample” from people exiting the polling places. Ask 10 people. Assume they are chosen systematically. • 6 for Liar; 4 for Dummy • Odds that the sample incorrectly identifies the real winner? high

  18. Example (cont’d) • Researcher knows that his sample may not paint an accurate picture. • More likely to be accurate if he picks a larger sample. Say 30. Or 100. • When statistically significant? • When only 1 chance in 20 (5%) that his sample identifies the wrong winner: (P = .05) )

  19. Example (again) • “Confidence intervals” becoming more common that “P” score. • Example: Liar leads Dummy, 56-44%. • Poller says 95% confident that this is accurately reflects the citizens of New Pleasant, within 3% in either direction. (“plus or minus 3%”= C.I.) • L’s lead is more than 3%. • So less that 1 chance in 20 that random error led us to pick the wrong winner.

  20. Medical example • Researchers find a relationship between Toxifam and lung cancer. • Sampled workers using Toxifam • sampled worker not using it. • Found 6% of T users get lung cancer • Found 2% of others get lung cancer. • Statistically significant? • If less than 1 chance in twenty that the samples chosen misrepresented reality due to chance.

  21. Nonrandom biases • D can also question studies that appear to have nonrandom flaws • Toxifam workers smoke at higher rate than the general public. That may explain their cancer. • Did the study factor this out?. • Voters were all polled between 7am and 10 am at one polling place close to Poller’s hotel. • Not a representative sample of community.

  22. In the courts • Test must be statistically significant • even though 95% confidence is arguably a more demanding test than “preponderance of the evidence” • Nonrandom biases • if serious could keep study out of evidence • otherwise, go to weight it is given by jury.

  23. Did Toxifam cause P’s cancer? • Can his doctor tell the cause? • If not, what are the odds is was Toxifam? • Recall the study: • 6% of T-workers get lung cancer • 2% of others. • Most likely cause? How do you know? • What’s the minimum % that would prove this?

  24. Advanced Points • T-3 workers at the Factory get lung cancer. • Can they prove causation? • How many will recover if they can prove negligence or defective product? • How many of the cases did T cause? • This paradox can also favor D. For example of T-workers have a 3% cancer rate. None recover despite 50% increase in risk. (Need a doubling dose; I.e. 101% increase).

  25. Revisiting Probabilistic Proof • Courts do accept probabilistic proof to prove that • the alleged toxin causes harm, and • the toxin is the likely cause of P’s injuries • Factors that may explain judicial willingness to accept this proof • we have identified a tortious defendant (unlike the “whose bus” case) • no other way to prove causation (not lazy P)

More Related