380 likes | 574 Views
Toddler Screening for Autism Spectrum Disorders: The Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT). Diana L. Robins, Ph.D. Georgia State University drobins@gsu.edu www.mchatscreen.com. Pervasive Developmental Disorders. Autistic Disorder (autism) Asperger’s Disorder Rett’s Disorder
E N D
Toddler Screening for Autism Spectrum Disorders:The Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT) Diana L. Robins, Ph.D. Georgia State University drobins@gsu.edu www.mchatscreen.com
Pervasive Developmental Disorders • Autistic Disorder (autism) • Asperger’s Disorder • Rett’s Disorder • Childhood Disintegrative Disorder • Pervasive Developmental Disorder, Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS) • Collectively called the autism spectrum or the PDD spectrum
Early Deficits in Language & Communication Impairments in Reciprocal Social Interaction Restrictive, Repetitive, Stereotyped Behavior DSM-IV Diagnostic Criteria for Autism
Autism Criteria: Social Impairment • Marked impairment in the use of multiple nonverbal behaviors such as eye-to-eye gaze, facial expression, body postures, and gestures to regulate social interaction • Failure to develop peer relationships appropriate to developmental level • Lack of spontaneous seeking to share enjoyment, interests, or achievements with other people (e.g., by a lack of showing, bringing, or pointing out objects of interest) • Lack of social or emotional reciprocity
Red Flags in Toddlers: Social • Lack of pointing (esp. to declare interest) • Reduced joint attention • Failure to orient to parent’s face • Reduced response to name or voice • Lack of interest in peers • Failure to brings things to show parent • Reduced eye contact
Autism Criteria: Communication Deficits • Delay in, or total lack of, the development of spoken language (not accompanied by an attempt to compensate through alternative modes of communication such as gesture or mime) • In individuals with adequate speech, marked impairment in the ability to initiate or sustain a conversation with others • Stereotyped and repetitive use oflanguage or idiosyncratic language • Lack of varied, spontaneous make-believe play or social imitative play appropriate to developmental level
Red Flags in Toddlers: Communication • Delay in, or total lack of, the development of spoken language is the MOST COMMON PRESENTING CONCERN, but not specific to ASD • Stereotyped and repetitive use of language or idiosyncratic language
Red Flags in Toddlers: Play • Limited play skills • Reduced or absent pretend play • Reduced or absent imitative play
Autism Criteria: Restricted, Repetitive, Stereotyped Behaviors, Interests, & Activities • Encompassing preoccupation with one or more stereotyped and restricted patterns of interest that is abnormal either in intensity or focus • Apparently inflexible adherence to specific, nonfunctional routines or rituals • Stereotyped and repetitive motor mannerisms (e.g., hand or finger flapping or twisting, or complex whole-body movements) • Persistent preoccupation with parts of objects
Red Flags in Toddlers: Restricted, Repetitive, Stereotyped Behaviors, Interests, & Activities • Often emerge later than symptoms in the social and communication domains • When present in toddlers, generally the lower-order, or less sophisticated behaviors, rather than preoccupations and rituals, which may require more cognitive skills
Impairment(s) in Reciprocal Social Interaction AND one of the following: • Early Deficit in Language & Communication OR • Restrictive, Repetitive, Stereotyped Behavior Diagnostic Criteria for Pervasive Developmental Disorder, Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-NOS)
Motivation for Early Detection • Severity of diagnosis • Outcome improved by early intervention(Baird et al., 2001; Bryson, Rogers, & Fombonne, 2003; Dawson, Ashman, & Carver, 2000; Lord, 1995; Prizant & Wetherby, 1988) • Practice Parameters(Filipek et al., 1999, 2000) emphasized need for improved early detection
Delay in Diagnosis Birth 12 mos 24 mos 36 mos { { ? Parents first concerned (15-22 mos) Child seen by specialist (20-27 mos) Often further delay until definitive diagnosis
ASD-specific factors that influence the success of early detection • Heterogeneity in presentation • Physicians’ time with children is brief Absence of typical behavior is challenging to detect Motor milestones are usually preserved Positive signs of ASD may develop later than social and communication deficits
AAP Screening Guidelines • 2006 AAP Policy Statement(Pediatrics 118, 405-420) • Surveillance at all well-child visits • Broad developmental screening at 9, 18, and 24/30 months • ASD-specific screening at 18 months • Gupta et al. (2007) comment on Policy Statement (Pediatrics, 119, 152-153) • ASD-specific screening at 18 and 24 months • 2007 AAP Clinical Report(Johnson et al., Pediatrics 120, 1183-1215) • recap of ASD screening recommendations
Need for Screening Tools • Standardized • Supplement professional observation or surveillance • Clinical impressions are not sufficient (Johnson, 2007) • Clear algorithms for referral to specialists for diagnostic evaluation are expected to reduce age of diagnosis, and facilitate onset of early intervention services
Available English-Language Toddler Screening Instruments • Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (CHAT; Baron-Cohen et al., 1992, 1996) • Pervasive Developmental Disorders Screening Test-II (PDDST-II; Siegel, 2004) • Screening Tool for Autism in Two-Year-Olds (STAT; Stone et al., 2000, 2004) • Social Communication Questionnaire (Rutter, Bailey, & Lord, 2003)
Available English-Language Toddler Screening Instruments, cont. • Autism Observation Scale for Infants (Zwaigenbaum et al., 2008) • Systematic Observation of Red Flags (Wetherby et al. 2004) • Developmental Behavior Checklist, Early Screen (Gray et al., 2005) • Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (Allison et al., 2008) • Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT; Robins et al., 1999, 2001)
M-CHAT (Robins, Fein, & Barton, 1999) • Eliminated CHAT observation section • Expanded CHAT parent report section • Literature • Clinical judgment • Age range: 16-30 months • Administration time: 5-10 minutes • Goal: Identify all ASD, not just autism • 2nd goal after Baird et al., 2000 CHAT paper published: improve sensitivity
How to Score the M-CHAT • For all items except 11, 18, 20, & 22 a response of NO is a screen positive response • Items 2, 7, 9, 13, 14, 15 are critical • A child screens positive if the critical score is 2 or more OR if the total score is 3 or more • Scoring instructions, template, and Excel scoring program available for download from www.mchatscreen.com
Overview of the M-CHAT Research in Multiple Low-Risk (Primary Care) Samples
Total Screened (N=10,837) Not at Risk AT RISK Need Interview (N=899) = 6-10% Pass (N=9938) Not at Risk AT RISK Evaluation (N=132 +29*) Declined/Excluded (N=169) = 16-24% Pass (N=598) Non-ASD (N=54) ASD (N=50) Declined (N=57) Kleinman et al., 2008; Pandey et al., 2008; Robins, 2008
Psychometric Properties: Sensitivity • Ability to detect illness when truly present • True positives/all ASD in sample • True positives/True positives + misses • TP/(TP+FN)
Psychometric Properties: Positive Predictive Value • Likelihood that positive result is a true positive case; Confidence that screen positive means significant risk of ASD • True positives/all screen positives • TP/(TP+FP)
Other Findings • Prevalence in this sample: 1 in 217 • Most of the remaining 54 children flagged by M-CHAT + Interview had significant language or global developmental delays (6 typically developing) • Cases who passed the M-CHAT but were flagged by the pediatrician did not improve detection of ASD
Effects of Maternal Education on M-CHAT Screening % Failed M-CHAT p=.001 ≥ Bachelor’s deg (n=380) < Bachelor’s deg (n=376) Zaj et al., 2007
Maternal Education, cont. % Failed Follow-up Interview p=.000 ≥ Bachelor’s deg (n=380) < Bachelor’s deg (n=376) Zaj et al., 2007
Follow-up at age 4 • 1416 re-screened to date • Only two possible missed cases detected so far • 75% ASD cases retain diagnosis • 25% no longer have ASD, although 60% of the “recovered” cases continue to have other mental health problems (Kleinman, Robins et al., 2008)
Psychometric Properties of the M-CHAT • Sensitivity high, estimated in the 80-90% range • Specificity mid-high 90s • PPV of M-CHAT alone is low (.1-.4) • PPV of M-CHAT + Interview is moderate (.5-.6) Kleinman et al., 2008; Pandey et al., 2008; Robins, 2008
Goals of the Ongoing M-CHAT Study • Screen an additional 20,000 children over the next 4-5 years (half in metro-Atlanta, half in Connecticut) • Refine the M-CHAT • Better characterize ASD in toddlers to facilitate early detection of ASDs & early intervention
Practical Issues in Screening for ASD • Who to screen: everyone • When to screen: 18 and 24-month well-child visits, PLUS other ages when surveillance indicates ASD concerns • How to screen: use a standardized, validated instrument
Incorporate Screening in the Primary Care Setting • Parents complete screen prior to, or at beginning of, well-child check-up • Physician or other healthcare professional reviews results during visit • Respond immediately to screen positive cases
How to Respond to a Screen Positive Case • Refer to a specialist for diagnosis • Psychologist • Developmental pediatrician • Autism centers • Refer to early intervention • Babies Can’t Wait • Private providers • Coordinate care
Georgia State University Diana L. Robins, Ph.D. • Current postdocs, grad students and postbacs: • Sharlet Anderson • Margaret Banks • Julia Juechter • Meena Khowaja • Susan McManus • Kimberly Oliver • Vivian Piazza • Agata Rozga • Noelle Santorelli • Jamie Zaj Current Undergrads: Rebecca Bosch, April Coignard,Lora Henderson, Mirjana Ivanisevic, Amy Lasher, Molly Locklear, Robert Rivera, Janice Taylor, Sheniece Willis, Amber Wimsatt, Shelly Zody Previous lab members who contributed to the M-CHAT study: Assata Abayomi, Lyntovia Ashe, Nicolle Angeli, Jasmine Brigham, Laura Burch, Esther Choi, Leo Eng, Lama Farran, Wendy Greenway, Shelley Hinkle, Sean Hirt, Margaret Jones, Puja Joshi, Amy Lasher, Cassie Lovett, Melissa Nikolic, Christina Parfene, Ashley Proctor, Corey Reed, Ali Scott, Catherine Shelton, Gina Vanegas, Lisa Wiggins Center for Behavioral Neuroscience, NSF Agreement # IBN-9876754 2R01HD 035612, R01HD 039961, GSU-CDC Seed Grant
Deborah Fein, Ph.D. Marianne Barton, Ph.D. James Green, Ph.D. Thyde Dumont-Mathieu, M.D. Hilary C. Boorstein, B.A. Pamela Ventola, Ph.D. Emma L. Esser, M.A. Sarah Hodgson, Ph.D. Jamie Kleinman, Ph.D. Gail Marshia, M.S.W. Juhi Pandey, Ph.D. Michael A. Rosenthal, M.A. Saasha Sutera, M.A. Alyssa D. Verbalis, M.A. Leandra B. Wilson, M.A. Eva Troyb, B.A. Katelin Carr, B.A. UConn Acknowledgements National Institute of Child Health and Development Maternal and Child Health Bureau National Alliance for Autism Research/Autism Speaks National Institute of Mental Health U.S. Department of Education UConn Research Foundation