1 / 22

Sources of Flexibility in Human Cognition: Dual Task Studies of Space and Language

Sources of Flexibility in Human Cognition: Dual Task Studies of Space and Language. Linda Hermer-Vasquez, Elizabeth S. Spelke and Alla S. Katsnelson. Stephanie Lidd and Jamie Tauber. Human Navigation Abilities . Two unique features Capable of dead reckoning Great flexibility

vea
Download Presentation

Sources of Flexibility in Human Cognition: Dual Task Studies of Space and Language

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Sources of Flexibility in Human Cognition: Dual Task Studies of Space and Language Linda Hermer-Vasquez, Elizabeth S. Spelke and Alla S. Katsnelson Stephanie Lidd and Jamie Tauber

  2. Human Navigation Abilities • Two unique features • Capable of dead reckoning • Great flexibility • Disorientation • Can reorient themselves after disorientation • How?

  3. Cheng Experiments (1986) • Tested rats’ abilities to find food after disorientation • Rats went to the correct corner and the geometric equivalent at equally high rates • Despite differences in non-geometric cues (e.g. brightness, pattern, and odor) • Conclusion: Rats reorient in accordance with environment shape (geometric module)

  4. Hermer, Spelke: Orientation Abilities in Children (18-24 Months) • Similar to Cheng’s experiment • Searched for hidden object in all white room, then room with one blue wall • Searched both corners equally in all white room • Failed to use non-geometric property (blue wall) as a way of navigation (like adult rats)

  5. Results for Cheng and Hermer &Spelke

  6. Hermer and Spelke: Adult Disorientation • Conducted same experiment using adults • Adults checked geometrically equivalent corners in white room • Were able to use the blue wall as a way of orienting themselves and finding the object (used non-geometric)

  7. Hermer (1994): Disorientation in Children (3-7 years) • Described position of the object as behind colored wall or to the left or right • Children 4+ were successful in describing the objects position behind the wall • Children 6+ were able to describe the position as ‘left’ or ‘right’ of the wall

  8. Hermer (1997) Spatial Language in Children 5-7 years • Tested children’s abilities to use spatial language • Correlation between spatial language and ability to use non-geometric landmarks to locate objects

  9. Causation Between Spatial Language and Development of Spatial Performance Possibilities • Three possibilities • Independent, but developmentally linked • Depend on a common factor • Development of spatial language produces change in spatial relations

  10. Experiment 1 • Similar to Cheng’s Experiment • White room and room with a blue wall • Tested again in each room with ‘verbal shadowing’: repeating a tape-recorded prose • Non-shadowing conditions had ‘white noise’ playing • Disoriented (spun around) before each trial

  11. Diagram of Chamber Used

  12. Results

  13. Experiment 2A • Visual Experimentation with 3 conditions • Rhythmic Verbal: repetition of ‘na’ • Rhythmic Non-verbal: tapping out pattern • Verbal: Repetition of prose • Computer program showing grids varying numbers of ‘T’s and target ‘L’ • Subjects had to determine presence or absence of target ‘L’

  14. Results

  15. Experiment 2B • Like Experiment One, with modified disorientation for last 8 subjects • Results: • Non-shadowing subjects produced higher accuracy in recognition • Shadowing subjects showed decline in accuracy • Subjects searched with high frequencies in correct and geometrically equivalent corners

  16. Experiment 2C • Identical with 2B except with Rhythmic shadowing • Results: • Shadowing subjects searched the correct corner with higher frequency than the rotationally equivalent corner • Higher accuracy than verbal shadowing • Non-shadowing produced same, accurate results

  17. What’s the Difference? • Rhythmic and Verbal were equally distracting, yet rhythmic was more accurate • Verbal shadowing may interfere with geometric and non-geometric combination process or • Combination process independent, but verbal shadowing may interfere with their ability to detect or remember the non-geometric landmark

  18. Experiment 3 • Verbally shadowing subjects with a simplified version of tests • Instead of being hidden to the left or right, was hidden on top of the walls • Walls were removed from chamber to remove the geometric cues

  19. Results • Subjects located the object with high accuracy • Landmark-appropriate search was higher in Experiment 3 than in the verbal shadowing conditions of Experiments 1 and 2b • Showed adults used non-geometric cues, despite disorientation

  20. Experiment 4 • Like Experiment 3, except the object was placed to the left or right of the wall • If verbal shadowing impairs the encoding of non-geometric information only in reorientation tasks, then both subjects should successfully find the object • If verbal shadowing impairs the conjoining of geometric and non-geometric information then non-shadowing subjects should find the object but shadowing subjects should not.

  21. Results

  22. Conclusions • Spatial memory system found in children and adult rats is present in adult humans • Language plays a role in the conjoining of geometric and non-geometric information

More Related