240 likes | 451 Views
Economic evaluation of health programmes. Department of Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Occupational Health Class no. 19: Economic Evaluation using Patient-Level Data II Nov 12, 2008. Plan of class. Collecting patient-level data alongside RCTs – some issues Analysis of uncertainty.
E N D
Economic evaluation of health programmes Department of Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Occupational Health Class no. 19: Economic Evaluation using Patient-Level Data II Nov 12, 2008
Plan of class • Collecting patient-level data alongside RCTs – some issues • Analysis of uncertainty
Issues with adding economic component to an RCT • Useful complement to efficacy data – very common in UK; and, relatively cheap • However: • Is comparison condition relevant? • Non-standard, more intensive measurement of outcomes that could affect results • Use of intermediate health outcomes • Inadequate follow-up period or sample size • Need to abstract from protocol-driven costs • Artificial rules mandated by protocol (selection of subjects, keeping them in study, adherence to Tx)
What to do? • ‘Pragmatic’ trials: • Patients representative of typical caseload • Routine follow-up • Meaningful and wide range of outcomes • Longer follow-up if relevant • Usually larger sample size • Unblinded patients and physicians • Not often done, particularly having a sample size powered for cost data • Modeling! • With adjusted trial data
Data collection methods • Add sheets to case report forms in hospital for hospital-based study • More or less detailed depending on what is being evaluated • Administrative data • Questionnaires and diaries • Accuracy of patient recall is an issue • Ideally monitor every 30 days
Statistical analysis of economic data • Nature of economic data • Skewed distribution (see examples next slide) • Missing data • Hopefully missing at random • Censored data • Difficulties with ICERs
Examples of skewed cost data For discussion: Why are health care cost data typicallyskewed?
2: If one of 3 interventions (each compared to a fourth one) is dominant, and all 3 ICERs are negative, magnitude of ICER is meaningless • Example: • A: (1 LY, - $2,000): ICER = -2,000 $/LY • B: (2 LY, - $2,000): ICER = -1,000 $/LY • C: (2 LY, - $1,000): ICER = - 500 $/LY • B is preferred yet is intermediate in value
3: X/0 = Infinity! • If difference in effects can be zero, with non-zero probability, then values will become impossible to calculate. • If confidence interval spans negative and positive difference, ICER becomes discontinuous: 0 Difference in effects
4: Ratios don’t work well as dependent variables in regressions • Often interesting to try to identify factors associated with cost-effectiveness!
Limits of hypothesis testing • Test H0 : No difference in effects • Even if cannot reject H0, this does not prove there is no difference (power of test could be low – high risk of Type II error). • Point estimate remains best estimate of difference • Mean incremental cost of £186 (95% CI, - £26 to £375) • Mean incremental QALYs: 0.007 (-0.008 to 0.023) • Decision-maker may be willing to accept higher risk than 0.05 of incorrectly rejecting null (may prefer to go with intervention even if not in fact more effective)
Incremental cost-effectiveness ratio Average cost per person: Experimental Tx (E) Control group(C) C E - C C E E - E C RCEI= Average value of effectiveness measure: Experimental group Control group (C)
Using the bootstrap to obtain a measure of the sampling variability of the ICER • Suppose we have nEXP et nCON observations in the experimental and control groups, respectively. One way to estimate the uncertainty around an ICER is to: • Sample nCON cost-effect pairs from the control group, with replacement • Sample nEXP cost-effect pairs from the experimental group, with replacement • Calculate the ICER from those two new sets of cost-effect pairs • Repeat steps 1 to 3 many times, e.g., 1000 times. • Plot the resulting 1,000 ICER values on the Cost-effectiveness plane See Drummond & McGuire, Eds., Economic evaluation in health care, Oxford, 2001, p. 189
An illustration of step 1 (Note: These are made-up data)
Going over the next steps again… • Do exactly the same steps for data from the experimental group, independently. • Calculate the ICER from the 2 bootstrapped samples • Store this ICER in memory • Repeat the steps all over again • Of course, this is done by computer. Stata is one program that can be used to do this fairly readily.
Bootstrapped replications of an ICER with 95% confidence interval Note: ellipses here are derived using Van Hout’s method and are too big; the bootstrap gives better results Source: Drummond & McGuire 2001, p. 189
Bootstrapped replications that fall in all 4 quadrants Source: Drummond & McGuire 2001, p. 193
A solution: the Cost-effectiveness acceptability curve • Strategy: We recognize that the decision-maker may in fact have a ceiling ratio, or shadow price RC – a maximum amount of $ per unit benefit he or she is willing to pay • So we will estimate, based on our bootstrapped replications, the probability that the ICER is less than or equal to the ceiling ratio, as a function of the ceiling ratio • If the ceiling ratio is $0, then the probability that the ICER is less than or equal to 0 is the p-value of the statistic from testing the null hypothesis that the costs of the 2 groups are the same • Recall that the p-value is the probability of observing the difference in costs seen in the data set (or a smaller one) by chance if the true difference is in fact 0.
Cost-effectiveness acceptability curve (CEAC) Source: Drummond & McGuire 2001, p. 195