180 likes | 460 Views
Some challenges
E N D
1. Untangling Speech and Language Difficulties in Toddlers? Bronwyn Carrigg & Dr Elise Baker, December 2009
On behalf of NSW EBP Paediatric Speech Group
2. Some challenges
Our question related to diagnostic evidence
Draft CAP-Diagnostic to committee (Dollaghan)
Requires reading of speech & language literature
3. Huge body of Late Talker literature underlies the topic (ie can we accurately diagnose language impairment in young children, let alone differentially diagnose speech vs language diffics?)
3. Quick Review: Late Language Emergence (LLE)
Subgroups of toddlers with LLE exist;
Secondary language impairment ass with DD, ASD, HL
Late Talkers (LT) no other difficulties or diagnoses, 10 -19% of 2 year olds (Klee, 1998, Rescorla, 2002, Rice 08)
LT - Normal variation will move into WNL range
LT - Primary speech/language impairment
- type of primary communication impairment?
ie language, phonology, sCAS?
4. The procedures most frequently used to differentiate
CN with Language delay from those with typical
language development are;
Parent-report measures of total vocab size;
- Language Development Survey (LDS Rescorla 98)
- MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory:
Words & Sentences (CDI:WS Fenson et al 93)
Parent report of 2-3 word combinations
Test battery measuring expressive & receptive lang
**Could looking at Speech Markers eg syllable shape, phonetic
inventory, or Nonword Repetition in toddlers, add dx info?
5.
Clinical Question: For diagnosing speech and
language delay in late talking English speaking
toddlers, are measures of speech production (e.g.,
syllable shape, phonetic inventory) and/or nonword
repetition as accurate as currently used measures
(e.g., CDI, vocabulary size)?
6 papers read by group on this topic.
6. Oller, D.K., Eilers, R.E., Neal, A.R., & Schwartz, H.K. (1999).
Precursors to speech in infancy: The prediction of speech and
language disorders. Journal of Comm Disorders, 32, 223-245.
Canonical babbling: infants production of well formed syllables,
often in reduplicated sequences bababa. Mean age of onset 6
months. Rarely begins later than 10 months. True for children
from low and very low SES areas.
Results:
Infants with confirmed, late onset canonical babbling had smaller expressive vocabularies at 18, 24, 30 months than controls. No sig differences in comprehension. Small sample
<50% who babbled later than 10 months had sig medical dx. They were excluded from follow up studies at 18, 24 months
7.
Highman, C., Hennessey, N., Sherwood, M., Leitao, S. (2008)
Retrospective parent report of early vocal behaviours in children
with suspected Childhood Apraxia of Speech (sCAS). Child
Language Teaching & Therapy, 24(3) 285 306
Method: Retrospective questionnaire completed by parents of 3
groups of children; sCAS, SLI, and TD. 20 children per group.
Questionnaire covered:
Developmental milestones
Feeding & dribbling behaviours
Vocalisations & babbling (eg frequency of sounds, frequency of babble, reduplicated babble, variegated etc)
8. Results (Highman, Hennessy et al 2008):
Preliminary support for notion of differences in
pre-linguistic vocalisations of children with sCAS.
2. Similarities between sCAS & SLI group cf TD group
sCAS & SLI groups differed on following measures;
35% sCAS group reported never babbled (TD SLI did)
2 word combinations later for sCAS than SLI & TD
sCAS group met some motor m/stones later SLI & TD
4. No differences between groups for feeding or dribbling
9.
Carson, C., Klee, T., Carson, D. & Hime, L (2003) Phonological
profiles of 2-year-olds with delayed language development:
Predicting clinical outcomes at age 3. AJSLP, 12, 28-39.
Phonetic Measures: including the number of;
- different consonants - different consonants in IWP/FWP
- % closed syllable shapes - diff consonant clusters IWP/FWP
Results: preliminary findings. 28 children. 13/28 followed up.
Children with language delay -LDS+, Mullen Scales, & MLU
significantly lower on all phonetic measures cf TD children.
the more delayed the childs phonological development at 2
years, the more at risk for continuing problems with language acquisition at 3 years
10. Mirak, J. & Rescorla, L. (1998) Phonetic skills and vocabulary
size in late talkers: Concurrent and predictive relationships.
Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 1-17
Investigated 37 LTs ( 24-31 months), comp WNL. 20 TD.
Relation b/w phonetic inventory & vocabulary size
CN with smaller vocabs likely to have limited phonetic reps
LTs >difficulty with medial & final consonants than initial
2. Predictive relation b/w these variables & language at 3 yrs
phonetic skills and size of vocabulary were not predictive
severity predicted language outcomes at 3 yrs, ie degree of expressive delay relative to age
11. Williams, L., & Elbert, M. (2003). A prospective longitudinal
study of phonological development in late talkers. LSHSS, 34,2
Provides longitudinal, in depth phonological data on 5 late
talkers. Seen monthly for 10-12 mths. 3/5 resolved by 33 mths
Possible predictive factors: quantitative and **qualitative
differences in phonological skills of those who did not catch up.
Extent of delay. Age at diagnosis. 2 unresolved cases, hx otitis
media. No info on comp, family hx). Preliminary study.
< phonetic inventory < diversity of syllable structure
> sound errors > variability & inconsistent
atypical error patterns slower rate of resolution
> simple syllable structure chronological mismatch
12. Stokes, S. & Klee, T (2009) The diagnostic accuracy of a new
Test of Early Nonword Repetition for differentiating late talking
and typically developing children. JSHR, 52, 872-882
Nonword Repetition: an unbiased Ax of language related ability,
requires psycholinguistic processing without relying on meaning
Could it be a clinical marker of SLI?
Could it be used as indicator of early language delay?
TENWR: 12 nonwords, 1-4 syllables, eg dafi, pirduluhmaip)
Results:
1-3 syllable NWR not useful in differentiating 2 groups
4 syllable NWR showed better accuracy but small sample size, poor compliance. favourable, preliminary results p879
13.
Clinical Bottom Line:
Clinical bottom line for each paper (CAP).
Combined clinical bottom line for the topic (CAT)
Affirm or change practise
Tells clinicians how to apply the available evidence clinically
Is informed by consideration of Level of Evidence, comments on design, and robust discussion among the group.
14. Clinical Question: For diagnosing speech and language delay
in late talking English speaking toddlers, are measures of
speech production (e.g., syllable shape, phonetic inventory)
and/or nonword repetition as accurate as currently used
measures (e.g., CDI, vocabulary size)?
Clinical Bottom Line:
No definitive study proving these measures are equally reliable or could replaced current methods of practice.
However, research suggests that measures of children's speech and non-word repetition ability that could contribute to the accurate diagnosis of speech/language delay in toddlers.
15. Clinical Bottom Line (contd):
The focus for many years has been on language of LTs. The
research suggests that SLPs may benefit from also considering
children's speech production skills (small phonetic inventories,
little change in phonological skills over time, small word shape
inventory), and their possibly NWR abilities (4-syllables words
preliminary findings).
16.
Additional Clinical Bottom Line Information
There is a relationship between measures of lexical
development (eg vocab size, word combinations) and phonetic inventory in LTs. They have smaller vocabularies and reduced phonetic inventories.
The predictive value of either measure not clearly supported over a number of studies. Preliminary investigations suggest qualitative phonological measures (with quantitative measures), and babbling onset and type are potential variables factors for further study
However, severity and age at diagnosis appear to predict language outcomes at 3 years. Older a child is when diagnosed and more severe (greater developmental lag), more likely child has true speech +/or language impairment.
17. Carson, C., Klee, T., Carson, D. & Hime, L. (2003). Phonological profiles
of 2-year-olds with delayed language development: Predicting clinical
outcomes at age 3. AJSLP, 12, 28-39.
Highman, C., Hennessey, N., Sherwood, M., Leitao, S. (2008).
Retrospective parent report of early vocal behaviours in children with
suspected Childhood Apraxia of Speech (sCAS). CLTT, 24(3) 285 306
Mirak, J. & Rescorla, L. (1998) Phonetic skills and vocabulary size in late
talkers: Concurrent and predictive relationships. Applied Psycholinguistics
19, 1-17
Oller, D.K., Eilers, R.E., Neal, A.R., & Schwartz, H.K. (1999). Precursors to
speech in infancy: The prediction of speech and language disorders.
Journal of Communication Disorders, 32, 223-245.
Stokes, S. & Klee, T (2009) The diagnostic accuracy of a new Test of Early
Nonword Repetition for differentiating late talking and typically developing
children. JSHR, 52, 872-882
Williams, L., & Elbert, M. (2003). A prospective longitudinal study of
phonological development in late talkers. LSHSS, 34, 2
18.
Thank you to EBP Paed Speech members;
SWAHS, SSWAHS, HNEAHS, NSCCAHS,
SESIAHS, University of Sydney, Private SPs
To join contact;
bronwyn.carrigg@sesiahs.health.nsw.gov.au