250 likes | 263 Views
Attention. Attention is about choice Choice can be governed by what’s out there in the world Choice can be governed by internal mental states For humans, which is it?. Attention is about choice. What should we attend to? What should we ignore?
E N D
Attention • Attention is about choice • Choice can be governed by what’s out there in the world • Choice can be governed by internal mental states • For humans, which is it?
Attention is about choice • What should we attend to? What should we ignore? • Some things are more relevant to our survival – threats, food sources, mates. • But how are we to know which things these are? • On the basis of physical qualities such as colour or direction of movement? • Or on the basis of abstract qualities such as name or category?
William James got there first • James (1890) • distinguished between two different systems: • Focal attention – controlled internally, by intentions and goals • Automatic attention – controlled externally by sensory events that demand attention • We’ll come back to this idea at the end
Choices governed by external world • This is the basic behaviourist view – our behaviour is controlled by things in the world. • The earliest cognitive models of attention adopted largely this approach • First important model was Broadbent’s (1958) Early Filter model.
Broadbent’s Early Filter model • Very large capacity sensory store feeds information through a filter into small capacity short term memory (STM) • The filter responds to physical characteristics such as colour or movement • Filter can be directed to take output of a given sensory channel (e.g., one ear or the other)
Evidence for Broadbent’s model • Dichotic listening studies in ’50s • Shadowing tasks: subjects listen to one track and ignore other track. Later can report on physical aspects (e.g., sex of voice) of ignored track but not on message content. • Broadbent (1954) – Split-span task • Subjects listen to headphones; separate series of 3 digits in each ear. Better at reporting by ear (65%) than by simultaneous pair (20%)
Sensory Register Selective Filter Short-Term Memory Broadbent’s Model • The filter mechanism selects on the basis of physical characteristics only
Broadbent’s Model • Notice the simplicity of the internal mechanism (the filter and STM) • Very little in the way of ‘moving parts.’ • There’s not much more complexity than in standard Behaviourist S-R models. • This model was in trouble pretty soon…
Problems with Broadbent’s Model • Treisman (1960) – meaning of messages can promote selection in dichotic listening task. • Subjects shadow one ear, ignore other. Story being shadowed switches to ignored ear. Most subjects then continue shadowing it for a few words before catching themselves and switching to ‘correct’ ear. • Continuity of story makes words in ignored ear relevant after switch.
Treisman’s (1964) Attenuation Model • Filtering is not all-or-none. Stimuli in SM can be more-or-less filtered out – and can be attended if they are very salient or very familiar. • Classic example – you’re talking to someone at a party and behind you, a person says your name – and you notice. • Notice that what happens inside head now starting to be important.
Problems with Broadbent’s Model • Moray (1960) – subjects detect their own name in ignored ear. • Gray & Wedderburn (1960) – 2 Oxford undergraduates; alternated words in a phrase from ear to ear. Subjects reported phrases intact. • Attended Ear Poor 2 Jane • Unattended Ear 5 Aunt 9 • Response “Poor Aunt Jane 5 2 9”
Corteen & Wood (1972) • First paired 3 city names with electric shock • Checked that conditioned response existed • Presented city names in unattended channel, during standard shadowing task • Stimulus GSR Frequency Shock-paired city names 38% Old words, not shock-paired 12% New city names 23% New words not in learning list 9%
Problems with Broadbent’s Model • Stroop Effect • Task: name colour of ink a word is printed in • Effect: RT is longer if word spells a competing colour. • REDYELLOWGREEN • PINKBLUE BROWN • According to Broadbent, should be able to attend to colour and ignore word.
Late Filter Theory – Deutsch & Deutsch • Deutsch & Deutsch (1963) responded to the many problems with Broadbent’s model by arguing that all stimuli are analyzed for meaning. • They suggested that selection happens only after semantic analysis, and means selection for awareness. • Problem is that while selection fails sometimes, it also succeeds sometimes. • But like Broadbent, D & D believed in role for Central Processor.
Central Processor • Broadbent’s version of STM was revised in Atkinson & Shiffrin’s (1971) Modal Model. CP made more complex. • CP now has many control processes: selection, encoding, rehearsal, and such. • All tasks require access to this central processor • Implication – if several tasks want access to CP at same time, should be cost
Problems with CP idea • Allport et al. (1972) – two complex tasks can be done simultaneously if they don’t share input and output modalities. • Shadowing task – aural input, verbal output • Playing piano (sight reading) – visual input, manual output • No cost to dual-task performance compared to when each task performed alone. • Cannot be only one “central processor”
Problems with CP idea • Spelke et al. (1976) – two complex tasks interfered originally, but after 85 hours of practice, no interference • Write to dictation – aural input, manual output • Read out loud – visual input, verbal output
More complex approaches • By end of 1970s, Johnston & Heinz were advocating a model in which person can choose either early or late selection, depending upon their purpose. • Johnston & Heinz (1978) • Dual task study, varying instructions • Showed subjects could choose either earlier or later selection, but later selection interfered with other processes.
Norman & Shallice (1986) • Supervisory Attentional System • Part of a model of control of thought and action • SAS involved: • in novel situations & poorly learned tasks • in dangerous situations • in troubleshooting • in tasks involving planning & decision making • when habitual responses must be stopped
Norman & Shallice (1986) • Model consists of: • Special purpose cognitive modules (e.g., for object recognition) • Schemas (programs that run on the modules) • Contention scheduling (resolves conflict when several schemas want to run at the same time) • Supervisory Attentional System (which activate or inhibit schemas to bias their selection)
Supervisory Attentional System Control schema Schema trigger database Effector System Contention scheduling N&S 1986 - Supervisory Attentional System Perceptual System
Norman & Shallice (1986) SAS Model • Multiple, autonomous, mutually competing schemas • Schemas are influenced by supervisory attentional system • SAS enhances one schema over others, allowing it to attain dominance and control behaviour • Compare to original Broadbent model for complexity of processes inside the head
Norman & Shallice (1986) • SAS model accounts for many facts about actual behaviour – i.e., action in the world, including slips and errors. • Capture errors – when familiar stimuli are responded to in wrong context • Crosstalk errors – interference between two tasks • Similar to errors caused by damage to frontal lobes
Current state of theory • Since 1986, idea of a single executive controller has become less popular. • Many people now say, idea was just a metaphor taken from politics or management, but brain doesn’t work that way. • Brain seems to work with many subsystems operating in parallel, but no single controller • Homunculus problem
Fractionation of executive control Current view inspired by much research in Neuropsychology and connectionism: control emerges from complex inter-actions among lots of special-purpose modules. • Control is not administered by some agent. • Allport (1989) • Logan (1985)