150 likes | 270 Views
Using School Choice Lotteries to Test Measures of School Effectiveness. David Deming Harvard University and NBER. Measuring School Effectiveness. School rankings, ratings, league tables Gain score or “value-added” modeling approach (VAMs) School VAMs now in ~30 U.S. states (Blank 2010)
E N D
Using School Choice Lotteries to Test Measures of School Effectiveness David Deming Harvard University and NBER
Measuring School Effectiveness • School rankings, ratings, league tables • Gain score or “value-added” modeling approach (VAMs) • School VAMs now in ~30 U.S. states (Blank 2010) • Teacher VAMs used in evaluation, retention • Accuracy of VAMs is important for incentive design and student welfare (Baker 2002, Rothstein 2010)
VAM Research • Large literature on measurement / technical issues • First order issue mostly untested - is assignment of teachers to classes within schools as good as random? • Kane and Staiger (2008), Kane et al (2013) randomly assign classes to teachers, test validity of VAMs • Chetty, Friedman and Rockoff (2013) use quasi-experimental design with teacher mobility • School VAMs require conditionally exogenous sorting of students across schools • Would you consent to this experiment?
Using School Choice Lotteries • Oversubscribed public schools in Charlotte-Mecklenburg • Random assignment, within a self-selected group of applicants • Estimate VAMs using data from prior cohorts • Vary model specification, sample, outcome • Out-of-sample predictions of “school effects” • Use VAM estimates to predict the treatment effect of winning the lottery
Data and VAMs • Grades 4-8 • Covariates - test scores from 1996-97 to 2001-2002, demographics • Models with nothing in X (levels), lagged scores only, lagged scores + demographics • Outcomes - 2003 and later test scores ; • Obtain by computing average residual (random effects) • Results are very similar with fixed effects • “School-by-grade” effects
Lottery Data and Sample • 2,599 students in 118 separate lotteries for “marginal” priority groups • Top 3 choices but nearly all randomization was over 1st choices
Empirical Strategy 2SLS with VAM in Fall 2002 school as the endogenous variable: 1 = 1st choice school N = neighborhood school = indicator for winning lottery j = lottery fixed effects (unit of randomization) If = 0.1, implied causal effect of attending school j relative to average school is 0.1. Thus if is unbiased, = 1. < 1 means VAM is upward biased… isn’t obvious - what is lottery applicant’s outside option? We try a few different things to check this...
4 Possible Explanations for Bias • Sorting on unobserved determinants of student achievement (Rothstein 2010) • Estimation error • “True” school effects may vary over time independent of estimation error • Lottery sample is self-selected, so treatment effect is different for them
No correlation between average test scores (in levels) and lottery impacts
Huge improvement from adding lagged scores (gains model) • Need 2+ years of data to fail to reject unbiasedness (triple negative!)
Adding demographics to increases coefficients modestly in all specifications • School effects “too small” when all prior years of data are included
“Shrinkage” • Attenuate teacher/school effects toward zero based on transitory variance in the estimate • Estimate a constant from noisy data • Empirical Bayes (e.g. Kane et al 2013) weighs all past years of data equally • Weigh based on autocovariance – teacher effects “drift” over time (Chetty, Friedman and Rockoff 2013)
Shrinkage improves prediction when only one year of prior data is used – with longer panel unshrunken more accurate • “Drift” adjustment is best here • Shrinkage overcompensates if there is true variation in school effects – CFR call this “teacher bias”
Concluding thoughts • Despite sorting concerns, school VAMs are surprisingly accurate • Best fit was gains only on full panel, but that’s not a theorem • Large standard errors admit non-trivial bias – need to replicate in other settings • Good news for policies that use VAMs, but: • Biases might be offsetting • Other outcomes are important too! • “School effects” may include contextual factors that are beyond school’s control (peers, neighborhoods)