1 / 17

Chapter 12 Acquiring Korean

Chapter 12 Acquiring Korean . Minjung Kim. Introduction. This chapter describes how typically developing children acquire the sound system of Korean S ound system of standard Korean Chronology of acquisition Speech error patterns. Korean Speakers in the World.

elise
Download Presentation

Chapter 12 Acquiring Korean

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Chapter 12Acquiring Korean Minjung Kim

  2. Introduction • This chapter describes how typically developing children acquire the sound system of Korean • Sound system of standard Korean • Chronology of acquisition • Speech error patterns

  3. Korean Speakers in the World • Official language of North Korea and South Korea • Spoken by approximately 78 million speakers • Sizable populations of Korean speakers in • China • United States • Japan • Former Soviet Union • Considered part of the Altaic language family • Agglutinating • Syntactic structure: Subject-Object-Verb • Lexicon • 35% native words • 60% Sino-Korean words • 5 % loan words

  4. The Sound Patterns of Korean: Vowels Table 12.1 Korean vowels • Diphthongs • Three semivowels or glides (/j, w, ɰ/) are combined with other vowels to create ten diphthongs (/jɛ, jɑ, ju, jʌ, jo, wi, wɛ, wɑ, wʌ, ɰi/) • Phonotactics of Korean suggest that these are true diphthongs rather than glide/vowel sequences

  5. The Sound Patterns of Korean: Consonants Table 12.2. Korean consonants Note: The different laryngeal types have been termed differently in the literature; “tense-aspirated-lax” or “fortis-aspirated-lenis.” In this chapter, the terms “lax-tense-aspirated” are used.

  6. Stops • Three types of Korean word-initial stops can be distinguished by three acoustic-phonetic properties: • VOT • Shortest for tense stops • Intermediate for lax stops • Longest for aspirated stops • fundamental frequency (f0) of the following vowel • Lower following lax stops • voice quality at the onset of the following vowel • Breathy after lax stop • Creaky (pressed) after tense stop

  7. Fricatives • Alveolar fricatives (i.e., /s, s*/) • Lax /s/ • frication followed by aspiration • Longer frication • Tense /s*/ • Frication followed by glottalization at the onset of the following vowel • Shorter frication Remaining consonants /m, n, ŋ, l/ • All consonants except /ŋ/ can occur in word-initial position • Only /p, t, k, m, m, ŋ, l/ can occur in syllable-final position

  8. Word structure • All syllables have a vocalic core with optional C onset and C coda • Onset C can be followed by a glide • (C)(G)V(C) • Only CG clusters occur within the same syllable • C sequences can occur at syllable boundaries • /kɑm-tɕɑ/

  9. Speech Sound Development of Korean Age and Order of Phoneme Acquisition • Compared to studies of English, fewer studies have investigate the development of Korean • General finding: Stops are acquired earlier than other consonants • Study of 150 typically developing Korean children ages 3, 4, and 5 years (Um, 1994) • All three types of stops and affricates were produced by over 90% of children at all three ages • Fricatives were produced correctly by 50% of children age 3 years and 60% of children age 4 and 5

  10. Table 12.3 Developmental patterns of Korean consonants by syllable- initial and syllable- final position (Kim and Pae, 2005)

  11. Study of word-initial obstruents in children ages2;6, 3;0, 3;6, and 4;0 (Kim & Stoel-Gammon, 2010) • Age of emergence: 75% of the children produced at least one correct production of the target phoneme • Age of mastery: 75% (90%) of the children produced the target phoneme correctly at least 75% of the time • All tense stops mastered by 90% of children age 2;6 • Aspirated and lax stops emerged by most children age 3;0 • Lax stops mastered by age 4;0 • Acoustic study of word-initial stops (Kim & Stoel-Gammon, 2009) • Lax stops are phonetically and phonologically complex, which could explain their late acquisition • Slightly aspirated in utterance-initial position but voiced intervocalically • Low f0 of the following vowel (same for lax affricates) • Lax fricatives acquired at age 4;0, before their tense counterparts • Increased articulatory complexity • The /s*/ is produced with longer frication that is often followed by glottalization at the onset of the following vowel.

  12. Vowels • All vowels are acquired by age 3;0 (Um, 1994) • Exception: some of the diphthongs

  13. Phonological Error Patterns • Korean children demonstrate some error patterns predicted by Jakobson’s universal theory • Substitutions of stops for affricates and fricatives • Other error patterns appear to be specific to Korean • Substitutions of tense stops for lax or aspirated stops (tensing) • Velar fronting is less common than in other languages as velar stops are acquired as early as alveolar stops in Korean

  14. Productions of 220 Korean children aged 2;6 to 6;5 to propose a preliminary norm of the phonological error patterns for the Korean-Test of Articulation for Children (K-TAC) (Kim (2006) • Whole-word errors (and age of suppression), e.g., • Reduplication (e.g., /sɑtʰɑŋ/ → [tʰɑŋtʰɑŋ], “candy”) (3;0) • Consonant harmony (e.g., /nɑmu/ → [mamu], “tree”) (3;) • Word-final consonant deletion (e.g., /tɕhɛk/ → [tɕhɛ], “book”) (3;) • Intersyllabiccluster simplification (e.g., /oks*usu/ → [os*usu], “corn”) (4;0) • Segment-change errors and age of suppression, e.g., • Tensing or deaspiration (e.g., /phodo/ → [p*ot*o], “grape”) (3;) • Velar fronting (e.g., /kʌbuki/ → [dʌbudi], “turtle”) (3;0) • Nasalization or stopping of liquid (e.g., /kolɛ/ → [konɛ] or [kodɛ], “whale”) (3;0) • Liquid simplification (e.g., /kolɛ/ → [kojɛ], “whale”) (4;0) • Affrication (e.g., /s*ɑl/ → [tɕ*ɑl], “rice”) (4;0) • Palatalization (e.g., /phodo/ → [phodjo], “grape”) (4;0) • Stopping of fricative or affricate (/tɕɑdoŋtɕhɑ/ → [tɑdoŋthɑ]) (5;0) • Stopping of liquids (3;0) • Interdentalizationof fricative (e.g., /sɑtʰɑŋ/ → [s̪ɑtʰɑŋ])

  15. Error patterns across different laryngeal types in obstruents (Kim & Stoel-Gammon, 2010) • Aspirated stops were produced as either tense or lax stops before age 3;6 • Lax stops were produced as either tense or aspirated stops in younger age groups but only as aspirated stops in 3;6 and 4;0 year-old children • Affricates • Stopping predominant with a tendency to preserve the original laryngeal type • Fricatives • Substitutions of [th]or [tɕh] for /s/ (stopping) were most common • Substitution of [th]for /s/ was more common in younger age groups • Substitution of [tɕh] (i.e., affrication) for /s/ occurred more frequently in older children • The similar developmental pattern was found for the target /s*/ which was replaced by [t*]or [tɕ*]

  16. Connections • Other languages • Chapter 10: French • Chapter 11: Brazilian Portuguese • Common trends across many languages: Chapter 9

  17. Concluding Remarks Korean has an unusual sound system in terms of variations in laryngeal types The three-way word-initial Korean stops and affricates are acquired by the age of 4 The two-way Korean alveolar fricativesare not acquired until after age 6;5 Further research in older children is required to understand the age of acquisition for Korean fricatives With regard to sound errors, both language-universal and language-specific error patterns were present

More Related