480 likes | 669 Views
Exploring Value-Added Across Multiple Dimensions: A Bifactor Approach. Derek Briggs Ben Domingue University of Colorado Maryland Assessment Conference October 18, 2012. Outline. Motivation: Value-Added Across Different Outcomes Coming to Decisions in a High Stakes Accountability Context
E N D
Exploring Value-Added Across Multiple Dimensions: A Bifactor Approach Derek Briggs Ben Domingue University of Colorado Maryland Assessment Conference October 18, 2012
Outline • Motivation: • Value-Added Across Different Outcomes • Coming to Decisions in a High Stakes Accountability Context • Longitudinal Item Response Data • A Bifactor Analysis • Value Added to What?
A Brief Aside on Vertical Scales • The original title of this talk was “Multidimensionality, Vertical Scales and Value-Added Models” • There is a simple bottom line on this: vertical scales are not needed for value-added modeling. It’s a non-issue. • Even for models that focus on repeated measures and growth trajectories, the approach taken to create a vertical scale will rarely have an impact on teacher or school rankings. • For details, see working paper by Briggs & Domingue, “The Gains from Vertical Scaling” • Vertical scales can play an important role in supporting inferences about student growth in absolute magnitudes. • For a critique of current practice, see Briggs “Measuring Growth with Vertical Scales” (in press) JEM
“Don’t measure yourself by what you have accomplished, but by what you should have accomplished with your ability.” John Wooden, Basketball Coach, 1910-2010
Our Theories and Intuition Tell Us Academic Success is Multidimensional According to Common Core State Standards, students who are college and career ready in • reading, writing, speaking and listening: • Demonstrate independence, build strong content knowledge, comprehend as well as critique, value evidence, use technology and digital media, understand other perspectives and cultures. • mathematics: • Make sense of problems and persevere in solving them, reason abstractly, construct viable arguments and critique the reasoning of others, model with mathematics, attend to precision, etc.
Previous Empirical Evidence The variability in VA by outcome measure is greater than the variability by model specification. • Lockwood et al, JEM, (2007) • MET Study “Learning about Teaching” (2010) • Papay, AERJ (2011) These studies focused on correlations between VA based on different tests within the same content domain (math, reading)
Combining Information about Value-Added: Two Approaches • Compensatory • Take simple or weighted average of value-added indicator across test outcomes • Classify teachers/schools on basis of quantiles of distribution or confidence intervals. • Conjunctive • Classify teachers/schools in i categories on basis of j outcomes. • Make rules that simplify ij decision matrix to k. • Ensure that no teacher/school is ineffective on a given outcome.
“Tests Measure Student Achievement” • An achievement score is a function of the content sampled from an instructional domain • Teachers/schools may vary in their ability to in teach different subject matter. • Agnostic about underlying latent variable. • Observed achievement is an estimate of a true score or universe score (G Theory) • Each achievement domain has a different hypothetical universe score. • Consistent with compensatpory and conjunctive approaches?
“Tests Measure Student Ability (θ)” • This is a latent variable perspective. • But math and reading “abilities” are poorly defined latent variables. • What is distinct and what is the same about these variables? • What if reading and math items are really just measuring the same unidimensional latent variable? • Spearman’s g? • Should this be the focus of value-added inferences?
A Novel Application of a Bifactor IRT Model Common Factor “g”? Math Knowledge & Skills? Reading K & S? Item 1 Item 2 Item 45 Item 1 Item 2 Item 54 … … Items from a Math Test Items from a Reading Test
Research Questions • Is “achievement” distinct from “ability”? • If we remove the influence that is common to both math and reading test performance, what is left? • Are the subject-specific variables substantively interpretable across grades? • How do the three “theta” variables from the bifactor model compare to the “theta” variables from successive unidimensional IRT models? • What insights does a bifactor model give us about different approaches to combining estimates of value-added across test outcomes?
Exploratory Strategy • Leverage longitudinal item response data to estimate six “theta” variables: UNIDIMENSIONAL • Math (2PL IRT) • Reading (2PL IRT) • Math + Reading (unidimensional 2PL IRT) MULTIDIMENSIONAL • Bifactor math (Bifactor 2PL) • Bifactor reading (Bifactor 2PL) • Bifactor g (Bifactor 2PL) • Examine the characteristics of each as a “measure” • Compare the use of these different variables as the outcome in a (simple) value-added model
Bifactor Model i = items, j = item specific factors, g = general factor Technical Details Software: IRTPro 2.1 (Cai, Thissen, Du Toit) Estimation Method: Bock-Aitken, 49 quadrature points References: Cai, 2010; Cai, Yang & Hansen, 2011; Rijmen, 2009; Rijmen et al 2008.
CSAP Tests in Math and Reading Math, Grades 3-10 Reading, Grades 3-10 Content Standards Reading Comprehension Thinking Skills Use of Literary Information Literature Subcontent: Fiction, Nonfiction, Vocabulary, Poetry Mix of MC and CR items Content Standards • Number & Operation Sense • Algebra, Patterns and Functions • Statistics and Probability • Geometry • Measurement • Computational Techniques • All 6 standards emphasize application of content for problem solving and communication. • Mix of MC and CR items
Longitudinal Item Response Structure: Students nested in Schools Source: Denver Public School District
Student & School Characteristics Across grades 5-9: • About 62% of DPS students are eligible for free or reduced price lunch services (FRL). • About 10% receive special education services (SPED). • Between 10-20% are English Language Learners (ELL) Across DPS Schools:
Students per School 40 Schools
Value-Added Model • Fixed Effects Regression • Pools Grade 6 estimates (middle school) and Grade 9 estimates (high school) • Outcome: One of the six “theta” variables created. • Covariates: • Prior grade achievement in same outcome, • Free/reduced lunch status • English Language Learner status • Special Education status • Grade 9 dummy variable (grade 6 omitted) • Empirical Bayes shrinkage estimators
Caveats • This is a very simple VAM. • Limited set of covariates, no school-level vars. • No teacher linkages, only schools. • Only a single longitudinal cohort of students. • No adjustment for measurement error • (Though we did examine possible adjustments. Results not shown here.)
Correlational Patterns Across Grades for Unidimensional Math and Reading Note how strong these correlations are even after 4 years. math lower triangle; reading upper triangle main diagonal: math/reading correlations
Bifactor Loadings (Grade 7 2005) Horizontal blue line at loading of .3
Bifactor Loadings (Grade 7 2004) Seems clear that something is amiss with grade 7 data in 2005 so we omit this grade in analyses that follow.
Marginal Reliabilities The bifactor and math and reading estimates are rather noisy estimates. Low reliability at the student level.
Correlational Patterns Across Grades for Bifactor Math & Reading math lower diagonal; reading upper diagonal main diagonal: math/reading correlations
Regression Results with Unidimensional Outcomes Note: Each outcome is standardized, so coefficients can be interpreted in an effect size metric. * p < .05
Regression Results with Bifactor Outcomes Note: Each outcome is standardized, so coefficients can be interpreted in an effect size metric. * p < .05
School “Effects” Distributions from Unidimensional vs. Bifactor Outcomes SD Math = 0.22 SD Read = 0.13 SD Comp = 0.18 SD Math = 0.11 SD Read = 0.11 SD g = 0.21 Note: These are shrunken VA estimates
Unidimensional vs. Bifactor Math SD Uni Math = 0.22 SD BF Math = 0.11 Note: These are shrunken VA estimates
Unidimensional vs. Bifactor Reading SD Uni Read = 0.13 SD BF Read = 0.11 Note: These are shrunken VA estimates
VA Comparisons: Uni Math, Uni Reading vs. g Value-added for math seems mostly redundant with value-added for g (r = .98); but looking at reading separately yields some unique information (r = .82).
Math vs. Reading VA within Method Bifactor VA by Subject Unidimensional VA by Subject
Relationship of VA with School-Level Status Variables If low correlations with these variables was considered an indication of a VA indicator that successfully leveled the playing field, the school effects associated with bifactor math outcomes would “win”.
Summary • When math and reading outcomes are quantitatively combined (VAcomp or taking average of VA across subjects), this is essentially equivalent to estimating VA for “g”. • Math and reading items tend to load strongly on g • Math items load weakly on math-specific bifactor • Reading items have moderate loadings on reading-specific bifactor • Evidence that math and reading bifactors are not just noise. • School fixed effects explain more variability in math/reading factors than in traditional unidimensional measures. • There is unique information about reading that would be missed if math and reading were combined.
Limitations & Next Steps Limitations: • No links to teachers. • No access to actual test forms (and items) that were administered. Next steps: • Examine loadings by content and process standards in test blueprints. • Do results generalize to • schools & districts throughout the state? • multiple cohorts of students? • other tests? • more complex VAMs (control for unit-level aggregates)?
Tough Conceptual Questions • What is g? • Is it sensitive to instruction? • Is it what we want to hold teachers and schools accountable for increasing? • If a test measures something beyond g, what is that something? Can it be distinguished? • Value-added to what?
Claims from the Smarter-BalancedLarge-Scale Assessment Consortium In the domain of mathematics: Claim 1: Students can explain and apply mathematical concepts and interpret and carry out mathematical procedures with precision and fluency. Claim 2: Students can solve a range of complex well-posed problems in pure and applied mathematics, making productive use of knowledge and problem-solving strategies. Claim 3: Students can clearly and precisely construct viable arguments to support their own reasoning and to critique the reasoning of others. Claim 4: Students can analyze complex, real-world scenarios and can construct and use mathematical models to interpret and solve problems. Should each of these claims be measured with a unique score? Should we expect variability in teacher efficacy on each? Or are all of these claims wrapped up in g?