460 likes | 603 Views
Cognitive Architectures and Virtual Intelligent Agents. Pat Langley School of Computing and Informatics Arizona State University Tempe, Arizona USA.
E N D
Cognitive Architectures and Virtual Intelligent Agents Pat Langley School of Computing and Informatics Arizona State University Tempe, Arizona USA Thanks to D. Choi, G. Cleveland, T. Konik, N. Li, N. Nejati, C. Park, and D. Stracuzzi for their contributions. This talk reports research partly funded by grants from ONR and DARPA, which are not responsible for its contents.
The Nature of Intelligence • Intelligence arises from computational processes • A variety of functional abilities underlie intelligence • These abilities are general yet benefit from knowledge • Humans remain our best examples of intelligent systems Progress toward understanding intelligence requires taking a position on two key methodological questions: • What theoretical framework should one assume? • What problems or testbeds should drive research? Cognitive science and AI, although distinct fields, agree on some core assumptions:
Frameworks for Intelligent Systems • Software engineering (e.g., Brugali, 2009) • Blackboard systems (Engelmore & Morgan, 1988) • Multi-agent systems (Weiss, 2000) Such integrated approaches offer clear advantages in terms of modular development and division of labor. But they are not the only way to create intelligent artifacts. Because intelligence involves distinct abilities, it is tempting to develop them separately and then combine them. This position is well represented in the AI community by:
Cognitive Architectures • makes strong theoretical assumptions about the representations and mechanisms underlying cognition • incorporates many ideas from psychology about the nature of the human mind • contains distinct modules, but these access and alter the same memories and representations • comes with a programming language that eases construction of knowledge-based systems A cognitive architecture is all about mutual constraints, and it should provide a unified theory of intelligent behavior. A cognitive architecture (Newell, 1990) is an infrastructure for intelligent systems that:
Testbeds for Intelligent Systems • Educational tasks (e.g., reasoning in math and physics) • Standardized tests (e.g., IQ, SAT, GRE exams) • Integrated robots (e.g., for search and rescue) • Synthetic characters in virtual worlds (e.g., for games) Each problem class has advantages and disadvantages, but all have important roles to play. In this talk, I will report work on synthetic characters that has driven our progress on cognitive architectures. We also need problems and testbeds that pose challenges to theories and evaluate their responses. Examples include:
Synthetic Agents for Urban Driving Insert demo here
Outline for the Talk • Frameworks and testbeds for intelligent systems • Review of the ICARUS cognitive architecture • Common and distinctive features • Representation and organization of memory • Performance and learning mechanisms • Synthetic agents developed with ICARUS • Complex synthetic environments • Lessons learned from these efforts • Plans for future architectural / testbed research
The ICARUS Architecture ICARUS (Langley, 2006) is a computational theory of the human cognitive architecture that posits: Short-term memories are distinct from long-term stores Memories contain modular elements cast as symbolic structures Long-term structures are accessed through pattern matching Cognition occurs in retrieval/selection/action cycles Learning involves monotonic addition of elements to memory Learning is incremental and interleaved with performance These assumptions are not novel; it shares them with architectures like Soar (Laird et al., 1987) and ACT-R (Anderson, 1993).
Distinctive Features of ICARUS However, ICARUS also makes assumptions that distinguish it from these architectures: Cognition is grounded in perception and action Categories and skills are separate cognitive entities Short-term elements are instances of long-term structures Skills and concepts are organized in a hierarchical manner Inference and execution are more basic than problem solving Some of these tenets also appear in Bonasso et al.’s (2003) 3T, Freed’s (1998) APEX, and Sun et al.’s (2001) Clarion. But only ICARUS combines them into a unified cognitive theory.
Research Goals for ICARUS • a computational theory of high-level cognition in humans • that is qualitatively consistent with results from psychology • that exhibits as many distinct cognitive functions as possible • that supports creation of intelligent agents in virtual worlds We have not yet modeled quantitative results from experiments, but we hope to obtain such fits when ICARUS is more mature. This would enable more direct comparison to architectures like ACT-R, Clarion, and EPIC (Meyer & Kieras, 1997). Our current objectives in developing ICARUS are to produce:
Cascaded Integration in ICARUS Like other unified cognitive architectures, ICARUS incorporates a number of distinct modules. learning problem solving skill execution conceptual inference ICARUS adopts a cascaded approach to integration in which lower-level modules produce results for higher-level ones.
ICARUS Beliefs and Goals for Urban Driving Inferred beliefs: (current-street me A) (current-segment me g550) (lane-to-right g599 g601) (first-lane g599) (last-lane g599) (last-lane g601) (under-speed-limit me) (slow-for-right-turn me) (steering-wheel-not-straight me) (centered-in-lane me g550 g599) (in-lane me g599) (in-segment me g550) (on-right-side-in-segment me) (intersection-behind g550 g522) (building-on-left g288) (building-on-left g425) (building-on-left g427) (building-on-left g429) (building-on-left g431) (building-on-left g433) (building-on-right g287) (building-on-right g279) (increasing-direction me) (near-pedestrian me g567) Top-level goals: (not (near-pedestrian me ?any)) (not (near-vehicle me ?other)) (on-right-side-in-segment me) (in-lane me ?segment) (not (over-speed-limit me)) (not (running-red-light me))
ICARUS Concepts for Urban Driving ((in-rightmost-lane ?self ?clane) :percepts ( (self ?self) (segment ?seg) (line ?clane segment ?seg)) :relations ((driving-well-in-segment ?self ?seg ?clane) (last-lane ?clane) (not (lane-to-right ?clane ?anylane)))) ((driving-well-in-segment ?self ?seg ?lane) :percepts ((self ?self) (segment ?seg) (line ?lane segment ?seg)) :relations ((in-segment ?self ?seg) (in-lane ?self ?lane) (aligned-with-lane-in-segment ?self ?seg ?lane) (centered-in-lane ?self ?seg ?lane) (steering-wheel-straight ?self))) ((in-lane ?self ?lane) :percepts ( (self ?self segment ?seg) (line ?lane segment ?seg dist ?dist)) :tests ( (> ?dist -10) (<= ?dist 0)))
concept concept clause percept Hierarchical Organization of Concepts ICARUS organizes conceptual memory in a hierarchical manner. The same conceptual predicate can appear in multiple clauses to specify disjunctive and recursive concepts.
concept concept clause percept Conceptual Inference in ICARUS Conceptual inference in ICARUS occurs from the bottom up. Starting with observed percepts, this process produces high-level beliefs about the current state.
concept concept clause percept Conceptual Inference in ICARUS Conceptual inference in ICARUS occurs from the bottom up. Starting with observed percepts, this process produces high-level beliefs about the current state.
concept concept clause percept Conceptual Inference in ICARUS Conceptual inference in ICARUS occurs from the bottom up. Starting with observed percepts, this process produces high-level beliefs about the current state.
concept concept clause percept Conceptual Inference in ICARUS Conceptual inference in ICARUS occurs from the bottom up. Starting with observed percepts, this process produces high-level beliefs about the current state.
ICARUS Skills for Urban Driving ((in-rightmost-lane ?self ?line) :percepts((self ?self) (line ?line)) :start ((last-lane ?line)) :subgoals ((driving-well-in-segment ?self ?seg ?line))) ((driving-well-in-segment ?self ?seg ?line) :percepts((segment ?seg) (line ?line) (self ?self)) :start ((steering-wheel-straight ?self)) :subgoals ((in-segment ?self ?seg) (centered-in-lane ?self ?seg ?line) (aligned-with-lane-in-segment ?self ?seg ?line) (steering-wheel-straight ?self))) ((in-segment ?self ?endsg) :percepts((self ?self speed ?speed) (intersection ?int cross ?cross) (segment ?endsg street ?cross angle ?angle)) :start ((in-intersection-for-right-turn ?self ?int)) :actions((steer 1)))
goal skill clause operator Hierarchical Organization of Skills ICARUS organizes skills in a hierarchical manner, which each skill clause indexed by the goal it aims to achieve. The same goal can index multiple clauses to allow disjunctive, conditional, and recursive procedures.
goal skill clause operator Skill Execution in ICARUS Skill execution occurs from the top down, starting from goals, to find applicable paths through the skill hierarchy. A skill clause is applicable if its goal is unsatisfied and if its conditions hold, given bindings from above.
goal skill clause operator Skill Execution in ICARUS This process repeats on each cycle to produce goal-directed but reactive behavior (Nilsson, 1994). However, ICARUS prefers to continue ongoing skills when they match, giving it a bias toward persistence over reactivity.
goal skill clause operator Skill Execution in ICARUS If events proceed as expected, this iterative process eventually achieves the agent’s top-level goal. At this point, another unsatisfied goal begins to drive behavior, invoking different skills to pursue it.
Skill Hierarchy Problem goal beliefs Primitive Skills Execution and Problem Solving in ICARUS Reactive Execution no impasse? yes Generated Plan Problem Solving Problem solving involves means-ends analysis that chains backward over skills and concept definitions, executing skills whenever they become applicable.
Skill Hierarchy Problem goal beliefs Primitive Skills ICARUS Learns Skills from Problem Solving Reactive Execution no impasse? yes Generated Plan Problem Solving Skill Learning
Learning from Problem Solutions ICARUS incorporates a mechanism for learning new skills that: operates whenever problem solving overcomes an impasse incorporates only information stored locally with goals generalizes beyond the specific objects concerned depends on whether chaining involved skills or concepts supports cumulative learning and within-problem transfer This skill creation process is fully interleaved with means-ends analysis and execution. Learned skills carry out forward execution in the environment rather than backward chaining in the mind.
ICARUS is a unified theory of the cognitive architecture that: ICARUS Summary includes hierarchical memories for concepts and skills; interleaves conceptual inference with reactive execution; resorts to problem solving when it lacks routine skills; learns such skills from successful resolution of impasses. We have developed ICARUS agents for a variety of simulated physical environments. However, each effort has revealed limitations that have led to important architectural extensions.
Creating Synthetic Agents in ICARUS Urban Combat Urban Driving MadRTS Rush 2008
Synthetic Agents for ‘Urban Combat’ Our experiences in Urban Combat provided some clear lessons: • Storing, using, and learning route knowledge • Learning to handle obstacles through trial and error • Supporting different varieties of structural transfer across problems Each of these insights has influenced our more recent work on ICARUS. Urban Combat is a synthetic environment, built on the Quake engine, used in the DARPA Transfer Learning program. Tasks for agents involved traversing an urban landscape with a variety of obstacles to capture a flag.
Start Start IED IED Source Start IED Target Transfer Studies with Urban Combat Experiments with ICARUS agents on Urban Combat demonstrated varieties of structural transfer across multiple goal-directed tasks (Choi et al., ICCM-2007). Transfer vs. Control Conditions
Synthetic Agents for Urban Driving We have developed an urban driving environment using Garage Games’ Torque game engine. This domain involves a mixture of cognitive and sensori-motor behavior in a constrained but complex and dynamic setting.
Synthetic Agents for Urban Driving • Keeping the car aligned at a constant speed • Changing speed to obey traffic signs and lights • Changing lanes/speed to avoid vehicles/pedestrians • Approaching and turning a corner • Driving repeatedly around a block • Delivering packages to specified addresses We have developed ICARUS agents that address each of these tasks in a reasonably robust manner. We have also demonstrated its ability to learn driving skills from problem solving (Langley & Choi, JMLR, 2006). The Torque urban driving environment supports tasks of graded complexity, including:
Synthetic Agents for Urban Driving • Indexing skills by the goals they achieve • Multiple top-level goals with priorities Recent work on Torque driving agents has led to additional changes to the architecture (Choi, 2010): • Long-term memory for generic goals • Reactive generation of specific short-term goals • Resource-based execution of multiple skills per cycle Our use of this complex environment has driven our cognitive architecture research toward important new functionalities. Our early efforts on urban driving motivated two key features of the ICARUS architecture:
Synthetic Agents for American Football We have also developed ICARUS agents to execute football plays in Rush 2008 from Knexus Research. This domain is less complex in some ways but it still remains highly reactive and depends on spatio-temporal coordination among players on a team.
Synthetic Agents for American Football Our results in Rush 2008 are interesting because ICARUS learned its football skills by observing other agents. This and similar videos of the Oregon State football team provided training examples to drive this process.
Synthetic Agents for American Football Our efforts on Rush 2008 have motivated additional extensions to the ICARUS architecture: Time stamps on beliefs to support episodic traces Ability to represent and recognize temporal concepts Adapting means-ends analysis to explain observed behavior Ability to control multiple agents in an environment We have used the extended ICARUS to learn hierarchical skills for 20 distinct football plays (Li et al., AAIDE-2009). These results utilized Oregon State’s image-processing system, which extracted objects and simple events from the videos.
Synthetic Agents for Twig Scenarios Most recently, we have used Horswill’s (2008) Twig simulator to develop humanoid ICARUS agents. This low-fidelity environment supports a few object types, along with simple reactive behaviors for virtual characters.
Synthetic Agents for Twig Scenarios • Innocents go from ball to ball, pausing to pick up any dolls they encounter nearby. • Collectors sit in chairs, buy dolls from anyone who offers at the right price, and secure these purchases in safes. • Producers stand by trees, produce new dolls, and guard them by blocking any other agents who approach. • Capitalists work in pairs, buying or stealing dolls from Innocents, stealing them from Producers, and selling them to Collectors. We have developed ICARUS agents for these character types to demonstrate their interactions with each other. This served as the final project for an ASU course on cognitive systems and intelligent agents (http://circas.asu.edu/cogsys/). We have developed a specific Twig scenario that involves four types of synthetic characters:
Reasoning about Others We designed ICARUS to model intelligent embodied agents, but our work has emphasized independent action. Yet humans can reason about the beliefs, goals, and intentions of others, then use their inferences to make decisions. The framework can deal with other agents, but only by viewing them as objects in the environment. • One might even argue that such deep social cognition is a key feature of human intelligence. Adding this capability to ICARUS will require extensions to its representation, performance processes, and learning methods.
Ongoing ICARUS Extensions To provide ICARUS with the capability to reason about others’ mental states, we are: • Extending its representation to allow embedded modal literals [e.g., (belief me (goal driver2 (in-right-lane driver2 seg6)))]; • Revising inference to support incremental abductive reasoning that explains events in terms of mental states; • Extending skill execution and problem solving to pursue goals that change mental states (e.g., by communication); and • Introducing learning mechanisms that make inference about others more efficient with experience. We are developing these abilities to construct conversational agents for emergency medical assistance in the field.
Synthetic Environments / Architectural Testbeds In an STTR collaboration, we are using SET Corp.’s CASTLE, a flexible physics/simulation engine, to: • Create an improved urban driving environment that supports a richer set of objects, goals, and activities; • Develop a ‘treasure hunt’ testbed in which players must follow complex instructions and coordinate with other agents; • Create interfaces to ICARUS, ACT-R, and other architectures; • Design tasks of graded complexity that can aid in evaluating architecture-based cognitive models. Together, these will let the community evaluate its architectures on common problems in shared testbeds. These will support both extended, open-ended operations and brief, hand-crafted scenarios.
CASTLE Screen Shots / Assumptions • Rigid body dynamics — solid objects, Newtonian physics, friction, joints, springs; no flexible objects or fluids • Z-buffer graphics — solid objects, textures, lights; no shadows, motion blur, or irradiance
In this talk, I presented ICARUS, a unified theory of the human cognitive architecture that: Concluding Remarks Grounds high-level cognition in perception and action Treats categories and skills as separate cognitive entities Views short-term structures as instances of long-term ones Organizes skills and concepts in a hierarchical manner Combines reactive control with deliberative problem solving In addition, I reported a number of ICARUS agents that control synthetic agents in simulated environments. These efforts raised theoretical challenges, led to extensions, and revealed insights about the nature of intelligence.
Time – + + junk-protein junk-protein + Lysosome Cytoplasm Fe – + + + ROS membrane-damage + + lipofuscin oxidized-protein + lipofuscin lytic-enzyme – + H202 H202 Other Research on Modeling / Simulation Qualitative system-level models of human aging (Langley, NIA-2009) Quantitative process models of ecological systems (Bridewell et al., MLj, 2008)