300 likes | 630 Views
Oppositional defiant disorder . a recurrent pattern of negativistic, defiant, disobedient and hostile behaviour towards authority figures, which leads to functional impairmentoften a precursor to conduct disorder, substance abuse and delinquencysingle cause or main effects models do not adequately explain pathways into ODDearly, multi-modal intervention is critical(Practice parameters, J Am Acad Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 2007) .
E N D
1. Externalising disorders ODD; CD; ADHD
most common referral
high level of impairment
poor prognosis
high risk of co-morbidity eg. externalising disorder ? risk of internalising disorder, converse is not true
3. Conduct disorder a repetitive and persistent pattern of behaviour in which the basic rights of others and major age-appropriate societal norms are violated
often
fights, uses weapons
cruel to people and animals
bullies or threatens
steals, sets fires
lies
truants, runs away
4. ODD/CD Empirical evidence supports distinction between ODD & CD (Fergusson et al. 1994) and between ADHD & both ODD & CD (Hinshaw, 1994) although there is considerable co-morbidity and overlap of symptoms
ODD often precedes CD (OR 4.0 in NY Longitudinal study)
CD often precedes Anti-Social Personality Disorder
5. Conduct disorder “I would there were no age between sixteen and three-and-twenty, or that youth would sleep out the rest: for there is nothing in the between but getting wenches with child, wronging the ancientry, stealing fighting”
William Shakespeare, The Winters Tale
6. ODD/CD - epidemiology estimates vary according to diagnostic criteria, age, sample, and informant
incidence and severity increases across development
ODD present in 2-16% population, relatively stableover time, but two thirds do not progress to CD
3-7% early onset, life course persistent CD (Fergusson et al., 1996; Moffit, 1993)
20-25 % adolescent limited CD (Moffit, 1993)
boys at ? risk for CD (3-4:1), less marked for ODD, but girls may be under-diagnosed, tend to use indirect verbal and relational aggression, including alienation, ostracism and character defamation, not captured by current definitions
social disadvantage ? risk
girls more likely to be adolescent limited, but if early onset, prognosis is poor
some evidence for generational increase, particularly in females
7. etiology - biological factors genes
Twin/adoption studies implicate genetic factors
temperament
increased incidence of “difficult” temperament
neuropathology
? incidence of minor physical anomalies - markers of deviant neural development
altered neurochemistry eg, reduced serotonin (implicates meso-limbic system), underarousal ? thrill seeking
EEGs show reduced frontal activation ? impaired insight/judgement
? Exposure to environmental toxins, infections, maternal substance abuse
neuropsychological deficits (may be more important in girls, and in boys without other risk factors )
lower IQ, PIQ > VIQ
language, including verbal memory; executive deficits, poor social cognition
8. etiology - family and social influences genetic/environmental interplay
Antisocial adults more likely to have children with predisposing difficult temperaments and low IQ and to provide poor social models and maladaptive family environments ie. shared genetic/environment influences
attachment/parenting style vulnerable vulnerable adult, difficult child - more likely to experience attachment difficulties and to elicit maladaptive parenting and conflictual interactions
socio-demographic factors
inverse relationship between CD risk and family income, positive relationship with family size, lax supervision, harsh/inconsistent discipline and parental tolerance of antisocial behaviour
9. peer influences “early onset, life course persistent”
aggressive, irritable, oppositional children alienate well behaved children and gravitate to similar, high-risk peers
but environmental experience reinforces, does not cause, early onset CD
“adolescent limiteds”
less severe subgroup, mimic peer behaviour to achieve status in societies with extended adolescence ie. association with deviant peers - a causal relationship
10. Conduct disorder subtypes early (ie < 10 years) versus late onset
early onset CD often preceded by ODD
CD onset tends to be earlier in boys with aggression or co-morbid ADHD
“adolescent limited” versus “life -long persistent”
overt (confrontational) versus covert
socialised versus non-socialised
11. Conduct disorder - explanatory models Robins (1991)
alienation, lack of attachment to conventional society ie. attitudinal problem precedes anti-social behaviour
Gottfredson & Hirschi (1990)
stable individual differences in antisocial propensity; core features – impulsivity, daring, high activity, low IQ
Moffit (1993)
two sub-types with different causal explanations
(i) ‘life course persistents’ reflect interaction of neurobiology + criminogenic environments
(ii) ‘adolescent limited’ explained by peer modelling
12. Conduct disorder - explanatory models Lahey et al. (1999) - an integrated model
Children at risk of CD exhibit key temperament dimensions of
oppositionality
aggression (high heritability)
low harm avoidance
callousness eg. lack of empathy, diminished guilt, ? need for approval
novelty seeking ie. arousal > inhibition
but
relative strength of causal influences varies across development
early starters - temperament + neurobiological factors important, environmental factors increasingly implicated with age i.e. magnitude of genetic influence inversely related, and environmental influence positively related, to age of onset.
13. Conduct disorder - prognosis 50% persistent disorder (Lahey, 1995; Offord et al., 1992)
poor outcome predicted by
early onset
severity of symptoms, particularly aggression or psychopathy
gender or age atypical behaviours
co-morbidity (CD + ADHD; CD + LD, CD + mood & anxiety disorders)
narrowing opportunities eg. school failure, dysfunctional peers, entry into criminal system, substance abuse)
Loeber et al., 2002
14. Conduct disorder - protective factors better prognosis associated with
higher IQ
better communication skills
adaptive parenting
close supervision
female gender ? more verbal, more empathic, less aggressive
15. Conduct disorder assessment low correlation across informants, but each contribute important information
teacher reports of ODD have highest predictive validity
self-report critical for CD
As pathways to CD are likely to be multifactorial, a careful history is critical
Structured, semi structured interview schedules help organise clinical information
16. Conduct disorder - treatment treatment difficult, prevention may be best option
need to minimise risk factors and enhance protective factors
Structured multi-modal approaches that address multiple risk factors are the most efficacious eg. Fast Track, LIFT
empirical support for
parent effectiveness training (Patterson, 1982; Dadds et al., 1987) vv
early identification/remediation of learning difficulties (Berrueta-Clement et al., 1984) v v
environmental change eg. exposure to conforming peers (Feldman et al. 1983) anti-bullying (Roland, 2000) v
child problem solving, conflict resolution, stress reduction/empathy building, social skills, anger management (Burke et al., 2002), v
pharmacology eg mood stabilisers (lithium), atypical antipsychotics, stimulants (Burke et al., 2002) v
“Innoculation” strategies such as boot camps unhelpful and may be harmful
psychotherapy unhelpful
19. Tom referred for firelighting
also long history
disruptive behaviour
aggression
school truancy/suspensions
academic “underachievement”
20. Tom early development
born small for gestational age
delayed language
unsettled, irritable
kindergarten
restless, unco-operative, aggressive “didn’t listen”
school
commenced school lacking in requisite social and cognitive skills
developed into an oppositional non-reader with conflictual relationships with peers and teachers, paranoid interpretations of social experiences
gravitated towards anti-social peers
21. Tom Family
intact family, father often unemployed
middle child of three
parenting fluctuated between being indulgent and punitive
father thought aggression was acceptably “male”
school failure explained as “laziness”
22. Tom Verbal scale
information 8
similarities 5
arithmetic 7
vocabulary 6
comprehension 5
Verbal IQ 78
Performance scale
picture comp 12
picture arrange 5
block design 11
object assembly 12
coding 9
Performance IQ 102
23. Tom Q. In what ways is a house built of brick better than one built of wood?
A. A brick house might have a fire-place
Q. Why do we have to put a stamp on a letter?
A. So that it gets to the person on time
Q. Why should you keep a promise?
A. If you don’t keep a promise, you forget it
24. Tom A lamp is straight and a candle is long
A cat chases a mouse
A knee and an elbow both stick out
You talk on a telephone and you listen on a radio
25. Tom Anna Thompson/of East Sydney/ employed/ as a cleaning woman/ in an office building/ reported/ at the Eastern District/ Police Station/ that she had been held up/ on Smith Street/ the night before/ and robbed/ of fifteen dollars./ She had four little children/ the rent was due/ and they had not eaten/ for two days. The sergeant/ touched by the woman’s story/ took up a collection/ of money for her.
Adrian Lesson was caught stealing $15 at the Police Station. She had two kids shivering and the policemen put her into gaol and the kids were safe.
26. Tom A complex interaction of vulnerabilities
genetic/family
inconsistent parenting
parental modelling of antisocial behaviour including tolerance of aggression
within-child factors
possible neurodevelopmental abnormalities
difficult temperament
weak language, verbal memory, sequencing and executive skills
Social/environmental
chronic school failure, social rejection
have all contributed to Tom’s diagnosis of CD
27. language deficits may
Reduce capacity for “self talk”, reflection, reasoning
impair self-regulation of behaviour
impair communication + socialisation
elicit negative interactions
increase frustration
28. children with sequencing deficits may
fail to derive meaning from environmental information
fail to link cause and effect
fail to connect actions with consequences
think illogically
become angry and paranoid when misinterpretations occur
perceive punishment as “unfair”
29. Children with executive deficits may
fail to focus on the salient
fail to engage in planful, goal directed behaviour “present-oriented”
lack strategies for problem solving
act impulsively
fail to monitor their behaviour and learn from mistakes
respond rigidly, fail to adapt to changing
circumstances
30. Children with verbal memory deficits may
retain only part of an instruction or explanation
lose meaning
misinterpret the message
respond inappropriately
appear oppositional and unco-operative
be punished for “non-compliance”
feel victimised
become anxious, paranoid or angry, leading to further
reduction in memory capacity
give up, act out