460 likes | 871 Views
Place of Articulation, continued. September 30, 2013. Administrative Stuff. Production exercise #1 is due at 5 pm on Wednesday. I’ve only received a few recordings so far! This Friday: practice transcription exercise on place of articulation. This has been posted to the course website.
E N D
Place of Articulation, continued September 30, 2013
Administrative Stuff • Production exercise #1 is due at 5 pm on Wednesday. • I’ve only received a few recordings so far! • This Friday: practice transcription exercise on place of articulation. • This has been posted to the course website. • For next Monday: another English transcription exercise • Broad and narrow • Phonetic features (dimensions of articulation) • Mid-sagittal diagrams • Let’s walk over a basic transcription problem…
A Useful Diacritic • Some English syllables have a consonant peak. • This can only happen with /n/, /m/, /l/ and /r/. • When this happens, the consonant is said to be syllabic and is denoted with a small vertical dash underneath. • Examples: ‘chasm’ ‘ribbon’ ‘eagle’ ‘feature’
An Interesting Fact • Some vowels are louder than others • dB of different vowels relative to (Fonagy, 1966): • : 0.0 • [e] : -3.6 • [o] : -7.2 • [i] : -9.7 • [u] : -12.3 • Why?
Another Interesting Fact • Some vowels are inherently longer than others. • Data from Swedish (Elert, 1964): • long short • high [i y u] 140 msec 95 • mid 155 103 low 164 111 • Why?
Sonority • Loudness is also a highly context-dependent measure. • Can vary wildly within speaker, from speaker to speaker, from room to room, and across speaking contexts. • However, all things being equal, some speech sounds are louder than others. • Course in Phonetics: • “The sonority of a sound is its loudness relative to that of other sounds with the same length, stress and pitch.”
A Sonority Scale • low vowels • high vowels • glides • liquids • nasals • fricatives • stops high sonority low sonority
Sonority and Syllables • An old idea (e.g., Pike, 1943): syllables are organized around peaks in sonority. • This is the Sonority Sequencing Principle (SSP). • Example: [bæd] is a well-formed syllable in English. • [æ] • [b] [d] high sonority low sonority
Sonority and Syllables • An old idea (e.g., Pike, 1943): syllables are organized around peaks in sonority. • This is the Sonority Sequencing Principle (SSP). • Example: [blænd] works well, too. • [æ] • [l] [n] • [b] [d] high sonority low sonority
Technical Terms • [æ] • [l] [n] • [b] [d] sonority peak high sonority low sonority
Technical Terms • The sonority peak forms the nucleus of the syllable. • [æ] • [l] [n] • [b] [d] nucleus high sonority low sonority
Technical Terms • The sonority peak forms the nucleus of the syllable. • The sounds that precede the nucleus form the syllable onset. • [æ] • [l] [n] • [b] [d] onset high sonority low sonority
Technical Terms • The sonority peak forms the nucleus of the syllable. • The sounds that precede the nucleus form the syllable onset. • The sounds that follow the nucleus form the syllable coda. • [æ] • [l] [n] • [b] [d] coda high sonority low sonority
Technical Terms • The sonority peak forms the nucleus of the syllable. • The sounds that precede the nucleus form the syllable onset. • The sounds that follow the nucleus form the syllable coda. • Together, the nucleus and coda form the syllable rhyme. • [æ] • [l] [n] • [b] [d] rhyme high sonority low sonority
Let’s Try This One More Time • If a liquid or nasal is in the syllable onset, it is not syllabic: • “reach”, “look” • If a liquid or nasal is in the syllable coda, it is not syllabic: • “fear”, “mall”, “form”, “cold” • If a liquid or nasal is in the syllable peak, it is syllabic: • “bird”, “worm” • “pull” (for speakers like me)
IPA Chart:Stops • You are already familiar with Bilabial, Alveolar, Velar • = the 3 most common places of articulation for stops • UPSID Database (in Maddieson’s Patterns of Sounds, 1984) • surveys 317 languages • 314 have bilabial stops (Wichita, Hupa, Aleut) • 316 have alveolar/dental stops (Hawaiian) • 315 have velar stops (Hupa, Kirghiz)
Palatal Stops • Peter says: • 59 languages in UPSID database have palatal stops • Palatals vs. Velars in Ngwo (spoken in Cameroon)
Also: Palatal Nasals • symbol: • not to be confused with the velar nasal: • PL: • Examples from Hungarian
Uvular Stops • Peter says: • 47 languages in UPSID database have uvular stops • Uvular nasal: • Peter, again: • Japanese: ‘Japan’
Quechua Contrasts • Quechua is spoken primarily in Bolivia and Peru.
Epiglottals, Glottals • There are no pharyngeal stops. • However, there is an epiglottal stop: • Peter says: • Check out Stefan’s epiglottis • There are also glottal stops: • As in English: “uh-oh”, “bottle”, “kitten” • More on these later
Epiglottals in Agul • Agul is spoken in Dagestan, near the Caspian Sea, in Russia • Note: no nasal pharyngeals, epiglottals, or glottals. • Why?
Back to the Coronals • Two parameters to consider here: • The active articulator • The tongue tip (apical) • The tongue blade (laminal) • The passive articulator or target • The upper lip (linguo-labial) • Between the teeth (interdental) • The upper teeth (dental) • The alveolar ridge (alveolar) • Behind the alveolar ridge (post-alveolar)
Coronal Basics • Coronal stops are usually dental or alveolar. • Dental stops are usually laminal • produced with the blade of the tongue • as is typical in, e.g., French, Spanish • Alveolar stops are usually apical • pronounced with the tip of the tongue • as is typical in English • Dental ~ Alveolar contrasts are rare, but they do exist.
Laminal Dentals • check out the labio-dental flap file
Yanyuwa Coronal Contrast • Yanyuwa is spoken in the Northern Territory of Australia • UPSID data-- • Languages with the following number of stop place contrasts: • 2 -- 2 3 -- 171 4 -- 103 • 5 -- 35 6 -- 6 <-- 5 of these languages are from Australia! • Yanyuwa has 7 stop place contrasts!
Retroflex Stops • Retroflex stops are produced in the post-alveolar region, by curling the tip of the tongue back. • Common in south Asian languages. • Peter says:
Two Places at Once • Labial-velar stops are not uncommon, especially in African languages. • Examples from Idoma (spoken in Nigeria):
Linguolabials • Linguolabials are formed by touching the blade of the tongue to the upper lip. • Examples from V’enen Taut, a language spoken in Vanuatu (the South Pacific):
Place Contrast Round-up • Most languages have three stop places: • bilabial • dental/alveolar • velar • If a language has a fourth stop place, it is usually • palatal or uvular • If a language has a fifth stop place, it is usually • retroflex • sometimes labial-velar