1 / 1

Discussion

COMMUNICATIVE ACTS AND WORD ACQUISITION IN TODDLERS WITH CLEFT PALATE. Sarah Boyce, B.A., Gerri Martin, B.S., Chariti Skinner, B.B.A., Kaley Wetherholt , B.S. & Nancy Scherer, Ph. D. Department of Audiology and Speech-Language Pathology, East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN.

hop
Download Presentation

Discussion

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. COMMUNICATIVE ACTS AND WORD ACQUISITION IN TODDLERS WITH CLEFT PALATE Sarah Boyce, B.A., Gerri Martin, B.S., Chariti Skinner, B.B.A., KaleyWetherholt, B.S. & Nancy Scherer, Ph. D. Department of Audiology and Speech-Language Pathology, East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, TN Abstract This retrospective study examined the rates of nonverbal communicative acts, canonical vocalizations, and word use in children with cleft palate as they transitioned from prelinguistic to linguistic levels. Fifteen children with cleft lip and/or palate were selected from a prior longitudinal study and their scores from the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (CDI) were collected from their evaluations at 18, 24, and 30 months of age. Ten minutes of the language samples were scored for the number of communicative acts (protodeclarativesand protoimperatives) based on Bates, Camaioni, & Volterra(1975), canonical vocalizations, and words. Results showed that the children with cleft lip and/or palate went through the transition from prelinguistic to linguistic level at an older age than reported in typically developing peers. However, their use of nonverbal communicative act and canonical vocalization rates were similar to typically developing children when they transitioned from prelinguistic to linguistic levels. Rate of word use was delayed when compared to reports of typically developing children. Statistical comparisons revealed no significant differences between rates of communicative acts, canonical vocalizations and words at prelinguistic and linguistic levels, although communicative acts and words approached significance. This preliminary study suggests that children with cleft lip and/or palate are developing nonverbal as well as vocal and verbal skills at a slower pace than their typically developing peers. Therefore, a prospective replication with larger numbers of children is warranted. • Method • Participants: • Retrospective study (previous longitudinal study of non-syndromic children with cleft lip and/or palate). • 15 participants (nine females and six males) from northeast Tennessee. • Nine of the participants had cleft lip and palate, while the remaining six participants had cleft palate only. • Procedures: • The children were evaluated at 18, 24, and 30 months of age, during which time they transitioned from the prelinguistic level (10 or less words on MCDI) to the linguistic level (more than10 words on MCDI). • At 18 months of age, 100% of the participants were in the prelinguistic level. At 24 months of age, 50% of the participants were in the prelinguistic level and the remaining 50% were in the linguistic level. At 30 months of age, 100% of the participants were in the linguistic level. • The researchers independently viewed ten-minute videotaped samples of each child while he/she was actively engaged with the clinician. • Data were recorded on the number of communicative acts, canonical vocalizations, and words the child produced at each age. • A communicative act consisted of the following: two out of three communicative means (eye gaze, gesture, or vocalization), directed towards another person, and displayed a communicative purpose (protoimperativeand protodeclarative) (Calandrella and Wilcox, 2000; Bates, Camaioni, Volterra, 1975). • The data were then converted to a rate per minute ratio. • Reliability: • Reliability for coding was calculated with a second reviewer watching 30 percent of each ten-minute sample. The mean inter-coder reliability was 98.3 percent (87-100%). • Statistical Analysis: • Statistical analysis was performed using paired t-test and SigmaStat Normality Test. • Correlation and effect size were calculated using the Spearman Rank Order Correlation and the Corrected D test respectively. • Results • Figure 1. Production of Communicative Acts, Canonical • Vocalizations and Words from Prelinguistic to Linguistic Levels in Children with Cleft Lip and/or Palate • The average rate per minute of communicative acts and canonical vocalizations increased substantially. • The pattern of acquisition was normal compared to typically developing children; however, the acquisition was at a slower pace. • Word use per minute increased less dramatically. • The children in this study were delayed chronologically in comparison to typically developing children. • Statistical comparisons revealed no statistically significant differences between rates of communicative acts, canonical vocalizations and words at prelinguistic and linguistic levels, although communicative acts (p=0.09)approached significance. • Discussion cont. • Proctor-Williams, Dixon, Brown, Ringley, Barber, & Light-Newell (2007) conducted a study using similar methodology with 29 typically developing children at 7, 10, 13, and 20 months of age. • Children in the current study (shown in Figures 2-4) demonstrated higher rates of communicative acts and canonical vocalizations at the prelinguistic and linguistic levels when compared to the children in the Proctor-Williams et al. (2007) study. • However, the children with cleft lip and/or palate exhibited lower rates per minute of word use. When age was compared, the children with cleft lip and/or palate were approximately 10 months older than the typically developing children. • Other studies of typically developing children have reported use of communicative acts at a rate of 1 act per minute at the prelinguistic level and 2 acts per minute at the linguistic level (Wetherby, Cain, Yonclas, & Walker, 1988). • The children in this study used 1.76 acts per minute at the prelinguistic level and 3.34 acts per minute at the linguistic level, well above the rate reported for prelinguistic and linguistic levels. • However, the children with cleft lip and/or palate were older than the typically developing children. Clinical Implications Assessment of communicative acts at the prelinguistic level may assist clinicians by assessing early communicative milestones that are less dependent on vocal production. Discussion Introduction Studies of early communicative development have shown a relationship between rates of communicative acts and canonical vocalizations with the acquisition of words for typically developing children (Yoder & Warren, 1999; Calandrella & Wilcox, 2000). Typically developing children are using communicative acts at a rate of 1 act per minute at the prelinguistic level (Wetherby, Cain, Yonclas, & Walker, 1988). The rate increases to approximately 2 acts per minute when children are using more than 10 words and 5 acts per minute when children reach the multi-word stage (Wetherby, Cain, Yonclas, & Walker, 1988). Delays in early speech and language have been reported for children with cleft lip and/or palate (Scherer & D’Antonio, 1997; Chapman, Hardin-Jones, Schulte, & Halter, 2001). It has been suggested that these delays are a direct relationship to the presence of an open palate until after repair at approximately 12 months of age. However, there has been little examination of nonverbal milestones of early communicative development in children with cleft lip and/or palate. If speech and language delays are related to the early unrepaired cleft, one would expect that rates of communicative acts would be similar to rates reported for typically developing peers, while rates of canonical vocalizations and word use may be delayed. The purpose of this study was to examine children's rates of communicative acts, canonical vocalizations, and words during the transition from prelinguistic to linguistic level. Comparison of Communicative Acts (Figure 2), Canonical Vocalizations (Figure 3), and Words (Figure 4) between typically developing children and children with cleft lip and/or palate at the prelinguistic and linguistic levels. Future Research Although there was no statistical significance between prelinguistic and linguistic levels of communicative acts, canonical vocalizations, and words, several comparisons approached significance. A prospective study with a larger number of participants is necessary to identify potential predictive relationships. References Bates, E., Camaioni, L., Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205-226. Calandrella, A.M., & Wilcox, M.J. (2000). Predicting language outcomes for young prelinguistic children with developmental delay. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 1061-1071. Chapman, K. L., Hardin-Jones, M., Schulte, J., & Halter, K. (2001). Vocal development of 9-month-old babies with cleft palate. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 1268-1283. Scherer, N. J., & D'Antonio, L. (1997). Language and play development in toddlers with cleft lip and/or palate. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 6(4), 48-54. Wetherby, A. M., Cain, D. H., Yonclas, D. G., & Walker, V. G. (1988). Analysis of intentional communication of normal children from the prelinguistic to the multiword stage. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31, 240-252. Yoder, P.J., & Warren, S.F. (1999). Facilitating proto-declaratives and proto-imperatives in prelinguistic children with developmental delays. Journal of Early Intervention,22(4), 337-354.

More Related